Social and Cultural Briefing Doc PDF

Summary

This document provides a briefing on social and cultural topics, focusing on social psychology concepts like attitudes and social influence.

Full Transcript

‭Week 5: Attitudes in Social Psychology‬ ‭ hat is an Attitude?‬ W ‭Attitudes are fundamental constructs in social psychology, representing evaluations of various‬ ‭social stimuli. Allport's classic definition describes an attitude as:‬ ‭Quote:‬‭“A mental and neural state of readiness, organized t...

‭Week 5: Attitudes in Social Psychology‬ ‭ hat is an Attitude?‬ W ‭Attitudes are fundamental constructs in social psychology, representing evaluations of various‬ ‭social stimuli. Allport's classic definition describes an attitude as:‬ ‭Quote:‬‭“A mental and neural state of readiness, organized through experience, exerting a‬ ‭directive or dynamic influence upon individual’s responses to all people, objects, and situations‬ ‭with which it is related” (Allport, 1935).‬ ‭Key aspects of attitudes highlighted in the source material include:‬ ‭Permanence:‬‭Attitudes are relatively enduring, though they can be modified.‬ ‭Evaluation:‬‭They involve positive or negative judgments of socially significant objects,‬ ‭groups, events, or symbols.‬ ‭Organisation:‬‭Attitudes are not isolated but form interconnected networks.‬ ‭Explicit vs. Implicit:‬‭Attitudes can operate at both conscious (explicit) and unconscious‬ ‭(implicit) levels.‬ ‭Functions of Attitudes‬ ‭Attitudes serve various functions, including:‬ ‭Knowledge:‬‭Provide a framework for understanding and navigating the social world.‬ ‭Instrumentality:‬‭Help individuals achieve desired goals and outcomes.‬ ‭Ego-defence:‬‭Protect self-esteem and manage threats to identity.‬ ‭Value-expressiveness:‬‭Allow individuals to express and affirm their values.‬ ‭The source material emphasises that the primary function of attitudes is object appraisal,‬ ‭providing an efficient way to orient oneself towards the social environment.‬ ‭Structure of Attitudes‬ ‭The source presents three key models of attitude structure:‬ ‭One-component model:‬‭Focuses solely on the affective component, the positive or‬ ‭negative feeling associated with an object.‬ ‭Two-component model:‬‭Adds a "state of mental readiness" to the affective component,‬ ‭suggesting a predisposition to respond in certain ways.‬ ‭Three-component model:‬‭The most prevalent model, including affective, behavioural,‬ ‭and cognitive components. This model acknowledges the interplay of feelings, thoughts,‬ ‭and actions in shaping attitudes.‬ ‭Attitudes and Behaviour‬ ‭While attitudes are expected to predict behaviour, the relationship is complex and often weaker‬ ‭than anticipated. The source explores various factors that can explain discrepancies:‬ ‭Methodological factors: Specificity:‬‭Attitudes and behaviours need to be measured at‬ ‭the same level of specificity for strong correlations.‬ ‭Aggregation:‬‭Measuring multiple behaviours related to an attitude provides a more‬ ‭accurate picture of the attitude-behaviour link.‬ ‭Personal factors:‬‭Competing attitudes, motivations, lack of perceived connection, and‬ ‭cost of behaviour can all influence the attitude-behaviour relationship.‬ ‭Social factors:‬‭Norms, presence of others, lack of alternatives, and unforeseen‬ ‭circumstances can all override the influence of attitudes on behaviour.‬ ‭Theories of Attitude-Behaviour Link‬ ‭The source details two key theories:‬ ‭Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA):‬‭This model suggests that behaviour is determined‬ ‭by behavioural intentions, which are shaped by attitudes towards the behaviour and‬ ‭subjective norms.‬ ‭Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB):‬‭This model extends the TRA by including‬ ‭perceived behavioural control, acknowledging that individuals may not have full control‬ ‭over their actions.‬ ‭Attitude Formation‬ ‭The source outlines several explanations for attitude formation:‬ ‭Behavioural explanations: Mere exposure effect:‬‭Repeated exposure to a stimulus‬ ‭can lead to increased liking.‬ ‭Classical conditioning:‬‭Pairing a neutral stimulus with a positive or negative stimulus‬ ‭can transfer the affective response.‬ ‭Operant conditioning:‬‭Rewards and punishments associated with a behaviour can‬ ‭shape attitudes towards that behaviour.‬ ‭Cognitive explanations:‬‭Self-perception theory: Individuals infer their attitudes from‬ ‭their own behaviour.‬ ‭Cognitive algebra:‬‭Individuals integrate information about an object using mental‬ ‭calculations, such as weighted averaging.‬ ‭Social explanations: Social learning/modelling:‬‭Observing and imitating the attitudes‬ ‭and behaviours of others, particularly significant individuals.‬ ‭Social identity and groups:‬‭Group memberships influence attitudes and shape‬ ‭individual perspectives.‬ ‭Ideologies, Values, and Social Representations‬ ‭The source expands on the concept of attitudes to discuss related constructs:‬ ‭Ideologies:‬‭Integrated sets of attitudes that form a worldview, defining goals and‬ ‭strategies for society.‬ ‭Values:‬‭Global, abstract principles that guide conduct and serve as standards for‬ ‭evaluation.‬ ‭Social representations:‬‭Shared understandings of the world that are constructed and‬ ‭communicated within social groups, influencing attitudes and beliefs.‬ ‭Measurement of Attitudes‬ ‭The source discusses various methods for measuring attitudes:‬ ‭Direct measurement (self-report):‬‭Involves asking individuals to express their attitudes‬ ‭directly through rating scales or questionnaires.‬ ‭Measures of implicit attitudes:‬‭Techniques like the Implicit Association Test (IAT)‬ ‭assess unconscious associations and bypass potential biases of self-report.‬ ‭Physiological measures:‬‭Monitoring physiological responses such as heart rate and‬ ‭facial expressions can offer insights into attitudes.‬ ‭Overt behaviour:‬‭Observing behaviours like interpersonal distance can provide indirect‬ ‭evidence of attitudes.‬ ‭The source stresses the importance of considering factors like wording, response bias, and‬ ‭social desirability when measuring attitudes.‬ ‭Conclusion‬ ‭ he source material provides a comprehensive overview of attitudes in social psychology,‬ T ‭emphasizing their multifaceted nature and significance in understanding human behaviour.‬ ‭Understanding the formation, function, structure, and measurement of attitudes is crucial for‬ ‭comprehending social interactions and influencing behaviour change.‬ ‭Week 7: Persuasion and Attitude Change‬ ‭ hat is Persuasion?‬ W ‭Persuasion is defined as "deliberate attempts by someone to change someone’s attitude". It‬ ‭encompasses persuasive communication aiming to influence attitudes and subsequently‬ ‭behaviour, particularly in advertising and marketing.‬ ‭Factors Influencing Persuasion‬ ‭The Yale Model identifies four key factors influencing persuasion:‬ ‭Source (who):‬‭We are more persuaded by similar, attractive, and credible/expert‬ ‭sources.‬ ‭Message (what):‬‭One-sided vs two-sided messages depend on audience intelligence‬ ‭and initial position. Matching the appeal (fact vs feeling) to the basis of the attitude is‬ ‭crucial. Fear appeals are common but effectiveness depends on intensity (inverted‬ ‭U-curve relationship). Repetition and message framing also play a role. The sleeper‬ ‭effect describes delayed persuasion due to dissociation of source and message over‬ ‭time.‬ ‭Channel (how):‬‭The effectiveness of video, audio, or written channels depends on‬ ‭message complexity.‬ ‭Audience (to whom):‬‭Gender differences are nuanced, self-esteem influences‬ ‭message complexity preference, prior beliefs affect scrutiny, and age effects are‬ ‭debated.‬ ‭Ultimately, persuasion is influenced by the interaction of source, message, and audience‬ ‭factors. For example, research by Debono & Telesca (1990) demonstrated how source‬ ‭attractiveness and message strength interact with self-monitoring levels to influence persuasion.‬ ‭Models of Persuasion‬ ‭The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) proposes two routes to persuasion:‬ ‭Central Route:‬‭Involves high elaboration and careful processing of message content,‬ ‭leading to enduring attitude change based on argument quality. This is more likely when‬ ‭motivation and opportunity are high.‬ ‭Peripheral Route:‬‭Characterised by low elaboration and reliance on peripheral cues‬ ‭(e.g., source attractiveness), resulting in rapid but less enduring attitude change. This is‬ ‭more likely when motivation or opportunity are low.‬ ‭Heuristic–Systematic Model (HSM) similarly posits two modes of processing:‬ ‭Systematic Processing:‬‭Involves effortful consideration of arguments.‬ ‭Heuristic Processing:‬‭Relies on mental shortcuts (e.g., "experts are always right"). This‬ ‭is often triggered by positive mood, time pressure, or low personal relevance.‬ ‭Both models acknowledge that individuals can switch between modes based on factors like‬ ‭confidence in their current attitude.‬ ‭Tactics for Enhancing Compliance‬ ‭Several tactics can enhance compliance with requests:‬ I‭ngratiation:‬‭Gaining compliance by appearing likeable and agreeable (e.g., flattery,‬ ‭name-dropping).‬ ‭Reciprocity Principle:‬‭Exploiting the obligation to reciprocate favours (e.g., free‬ ‭samples).‬ ‭Multiple Requests:‬‭Using a two-step procedure, where the first request sets the stage‬ ‭for the second, real request. This includes:‬ ‭Foot-in-the-door:‬‭Starting with a small request then escalating to a larger one.‬ ‭Door-in-the-face:‬‭Starting with a large, unreasonable request, then making a smaller,‬ ‭more reasonable one that appears like a concession.‬ ‭Low-balling:‬‭Securing agreement with a low-cost request, then revealing hidden costs.‬ ‭Action Research and Attitude Change‬ ‭Kurt Lewin pioneered action research, emphasizing active participant involvement in change‬ ‭processes. This approach highlights the effectiveness of group discussion over passive listening‬ ‭in changing attitudes and behaviours. Role-playing, where individuals argue against their own‬ ‭beliefs, can also induce attitude change, as demonstrated by Janis and King (1954). Successful‬ ‭examples of action research in health promotion include Australia's SunSmart campaign, which‬ ‭effectively changed sun exposure attitudes and behaviours.‬ ‭Cognitive Dissonance and Attitude Change‬ ‭Cognitive dissonance arises when inconsistencies exist between cognitions or between‬ ‭cognitions and behaviour, leading to psychological discomfort. Individuals are motivated to‬ ‭reduce this discomfort by changing their attitudes or behaviours.‬ ‭Festinger's classic cognitive dissonance model (1957) explains how individuals reduce‬ ‭dissonance. Examples include the study "When prophecy fails" where cult members who‬ ‭predicted the end of the world, when it didn't occur, rationalized their beliefs instead of‬ ‭abandoning them.‬ ‭The revised model by Cooper & Fazio (1984) includes the consideration of negative‬ ‭consequences and self-attribution of arousal as factors influencing dissonance reduction.‬ ‭Key dissonance-related phenomena:‬ ‭Post-decision dissonance:‬‭Regret or discomfort after making a decision, often‬ ‭resolved by increasing the attractiveness of the chosen option (Knox & Inkster, 1968).‬ ‭Justification of counter-attitudinal behaviour:‬‭Changing attitudes to align with‬ ‭behaviour that contradicts existing beliefs, especially when justification for the behaviour‬ ‭is insufficient (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959).‬ ‭Effort justification:‬‭Valuing an object or goal more after enduring hardship to obtain it‬ ‭(Aronson & Mills, 1959).‬ ‭These phenomena highlight the powerful influence of dissonance on attitudes and the various‬ ‭strategies individuals employ to maintain cognitive consistency.‬ ‭Resistance to Persuasion‬ ‭Individuals resist persuasion through:‬ ‭Reactance:‬‭Resisting attempts to limit personal freedom.‬ ‭Forewarning:‬‭Having prior knowledge of persuasive intent.‬ ‭Rehearsal of counterarguments:‬‭Being prepared with opposing arguments.‬ ‭Inoculation effect:‬‭Exposure to weaker arguments strengthens resistance to stronger‬ ‭ones.‬ ‭ ttitude accessibility and strength:‬‭Strong, readily accessible attitudes are more‬ A ‭resistant to change.‬ ‭Application of Cognitive Dissonance‬ ‭Cognitive dissonance principles can be applied to promote positive behaviour change. For‬ ‭instance, induced hypocrisy, where individuals are made aware of inconsistencies between their‬ ‭attitudes and behaviours, can motivate them to align their actions with their values. Morrongiello‬ ‭& Mark (2008) successfully demonstrated this by inducing hypocrisy in children to reduce risky‬ ‭playground behaviours. Other examples include promoting condom use by reminding‬ ‭individuals of their negative attitudes toward STIs or encouraging cleanliness by highlighting‬ ‭discrepancies between desired and actual home environments.‬ ‭Conclusion‬ ‭This document has provided a comprehensive overview of key concepts in persuasion and‬ ‭attitude change. By understanding the factors influencing persuasion, the different models of‬ ‭attitude change, and the strategies for enhancing compliance and resisting persuasion, we can‬ ‭gain valuable insights into the complex dynamics of human behaviour and apply these insights‬ ‭to effectively influence attitudes and promote positive change.‬ ‭Week 8: Social Influence‬ I‭. Types of Social Influence‬ ‭Social influence encompasses the ways our thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are shaped by‬ ‭others, whether their presence is real, imagined, or implied. Key types include:‬ ‭Conformity:‬‭Deep-seated, private, and enduring changes in behavior and attitudes to‬ ‭align with group norms.‬ ‭Compliance:‬‭Superficial, public, and temporary changes in behavior and expressed‬ ‭attitudes, often driven by power dynamics.‬ ‭Obedience:‬‭Following direct orders from an authority figure.‬ ‭Power:‬‭The ability to influence others while resisting their attempts to influence us.‬ ‭Sources of power include reward, coercive, informational, expert, legitimate, and referent‬ ‭power.‬ ‭II. Conformity‬ ‭Conformity involves aligning with group norms, which are shared beliefs about appropriate‬ ‭conduct. It can be driven by:‬ ‭Informational influence:‬‭Conforming due to the belief that others possess accurate‬ ‭information, especially in ambiguous situations. Sherif's (1936) autokinetic effect study‬ ‭exemplifies this.‬ ‭Normative influence:‬‭Conforming to gain social approval and acceptance, as seen in‬ ‭Asch's (1951) line judgment study.‬ ‭Referent informational influence:‬‭Conforming to norms that define oneself as a‬ ‭member of a valued group.‬ ‭Key Factors Influencing Conformity:‬ ‭Individual characteristics:‬‭High self-monitoring, need for social approval, social‬ ‭anxiety, and low self-esteem increase conformity.‬ ‭Group size:‬‭Conformity peaks with a 3-5 person majority.‬ ‭ roup membership:‬‭Conformity is stronger when group members are competent or‬ G ‭friends.‬ ‭Group unanimity:‬‭Even one dissenter reduces conformity.‬ ‭Culture:‬‭Collectivist cultures show greater conformity than individualist cultures,‬ ‭reflecting the prioritization of group harmony over individual expression.‬ ‭III. Obedience‬ ‭Obedience involves compliance with direct orders from an authority figure. Milgram's (1963)‬ ‭classic study demonstrated the powerful influence of authority, with 65% of participants‬ ‭delivering seemingly lethal shocks to a learner.‬ ‭Explanations for Obedience:‬ ‭Obedience to authority:‬‭Individuals are socialized to obey authority figures.‬ ‭Diffusion of responsibility:‬‭Participants attributed responsibility for harm to the‬ ‭experimenter.‬ ‭Gradual increase in demands:‬‭The incremental nature of the shocks made it harder to‬ ‭resist.‬ ‭Limited information in a novel situation:‬‭The unfamiliar context contributed to‬ ‭participants' reliance on the experimenter's guidance.‬ ‭Factors Reducing Obedience:‬ ‭Immediacy of the learner:‬‭When the learner was in the same room or the teacher had‬ ‭to physically touch them, obedience decreased.‬ ‭Immediacy/legitimacy of the experimenter:‬‭Giving instructions by phone, not wearing‬ ‭a uniform, or conducting the study in a non-university setting reduced obedience.‬ ‭Cultural Considerations:‬‭While Milgram's findings have been replicated across cultures,‬ ‭variations exist. Cultural dimensions like individualism-collectivism and power distance may‬ ‭contribute to these differences. As Ent & Baumeister (2014) note, "Obedience to authority is‬ ‭vital for the success of most human groups and organizations" and understanding obedience in‬ ‭a culture requires considering the specific social context and meaning of orders.‬ ‭IV. Non-Conformity & Minority Influence‬ ‭While conformity is common, individuals can resist group pressure. Non-conformity can lead to:‬ ‭Increased communication directed towards the non-conformist.‬ ‭Lower interpersonal attraction towards the non-conformist.‬ ‭Minority influence occurs when numerical or power minorities sway the majority. Effective‬ ‭minority influence relies on:‬ ‭Consistency:‬‭Maintaining a consistent stance over time and within the minority group.‬ ‭Investment:‬‭Demonstrating commitment to their position.‬ ‭Autonomy:‬‭Acting independently, not out of self-interest.‬ ‭Flexibility:‬‭Showing a willingness to compromise on less critical issues.‬ ‭Theories of Minority Influence:‬ ‭Conversion Theory (Moscovici, 1980):‬‭Proposes that minorities influence through‬ ‭deep processing and private acceptance, while majorities influence through superficial‬ ‭processing and public compliance.‬ ‭Convergent-Divergent Theory (Nemeth, 1986, 1995):‬‭Suggests disagreement with a‬ ‭majority leads to convergent thinking, narrowing focus, while disagreement with a‬ ‭minority fosters divergent thinking and broader consideration of alternatives.‬ ‭ ocial Identity and Self-Categorization Theories (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Turner,‬ S ‭1991):‬‭Minority influence is more likely when the minority is perceived as part of the‬ ‭in-group but poses no threat.‬ ‭Context-Comparison Model (Crano & Alvaro, 1997):‬‭Highlights the importance of‬ ‭various contextual factors, including the centrality of the targeted belief, the source's‬ ‭status (in-group vs. out-group), and the issue's subjective or objective nature.‬ ‭Conclusion:‬ ‭Social influence is a powerful force shaping human behavior. Understanding the dynamics of‬ ‭conformity, obedience, and minority influence is crucial for navigating social situations, fostering‬ ‭critical thinking, and promoting positive social change.‬ ‭Week 9: Social Groups & Leadership‬ ‭. Audience Effects‬ 1 ‭This section explores how the presence of others impacts individual performance. It delves into‬ ‭concepts like social facilitation and social inhibition, supported by key studies and theories:‬ ‭Triplett (1898):‬‭Found cyclists performed better with others, initiating the study of‬ ‭audience effects.‬ ‭Social Facilitation:‬‭Performance improvement on well-learned/easy tasks in the‬ ‭presence of others.‬ ‭Social Inhibition:‬‭Performance deterioration on poorly-learned/difficult tasks in the‬ ‭presence of others.‬ ‭Three key theories explain these phenomena:‬ ‭Drive/Arousal Theory (Zajonc, 1965):‬‭Mere presence of others increases arousal,‬ ‭leading to the dominant response. Supported by Zajonc, Heingartner, & Herman's (1969)‬ ‭cockroach study, where cockroaches ran an easy maze faster but a hard maze slower‬ ‭with an audience.‬ ‭Evaluation Apprehension Theory (Cottrell, 1972):‬‭Arousal stems from concern about‬ ‭being evaluated. Cottrell, Wack, Sekerak, & Rittle's (1968) study demonstrated social‬ ‭facilitation only with an attentive audience, not a blindfolded one.‬ ‭Distraction-Conflict Theory (Baron, 1986; Sanders et al., 1978):‬‭Attentional conflict‬ ‭between the task and the audience causes arousal, explaining why even non-social‬ ‭distractions can produce similar effects.‬ ‭Additional factors influencing audience effects include the audience's visibility, the‬ ‭performer-audience relationship, and task interactivity.‬ ‭2. Social Loafing‬ ‭This section examines the tendency for individuals to exert less effort in group tasks.‬ ‭Definition:‬‭"A reduction in individual effort when working on a task involving group‬ ‭effort."‬ ‭Possible Explanations:‬ ‭Coordination loss:‬‭Difficulty coordinating individual efforts.‬ ‭Motivation loss:‬‭Reduced individual motivation due to diffused responsibility.‬ ‭Ingham, Levinger, Graves, & Peckham (1974):‬‭Using a rope-pulling task, they found‬ ‭both motivation loss and coordination loss contributed to social loafing.‬ ‭Strategies to reduce social loafing:‬ ‭ maller group sizes.‬ S ‭Individual accountability through unique contributions and potential for evaluation.‬ ‭Meaningful and involving tasks.‬ ‭Belief in the group's effectiveness.‬ ‭Cultural Influences on Social Loafing:‬ ‭Gabrenya, Wang, & Latane (1985):‬‭Found American 9th graders displayed more social‬ ‭loafing than Chinese 9th graders in a tone-counting task. Chinese students even‬ ‭exhibited "social striving" performing better in pairs.‬ ‭Cultural differences in social loafing depend on developmental stage, task type, and‬ ‭group composition. For instance, both Chinese and US students exhibited social loafing‬ ‭in a sound production task.‬ ‭3. Group Socialisation and Norms‬ ‭This section explores group dynamics, focusing on stages of group development and the role of‬ ‭social norms:‬ ‭Group Socialisation:‬‭The process from considering joining a group to leaving it.‬ ‭Tuckman's (1965) Five Stages of Group Development:‬ ‭Forming:‬‭Orientation and familiarisation.‬ ‭Storming:‬‭Conflict and disagreement.‬ ‭Norming:‬‭Consensus, cohesion, shared identity.‬ ‭Performing:‬‭Smooth teamwork with shared norms and goals.‬ ‭Adjourning:‬‭Dissolution of the group.‬ ‭Levine & Moreland (1982, 1984):‬‭Proposed a model considering both individual and‬ ‭group perspectives in socialisation, focusing on evaluation, commitment, and role‬ ‭transitions.‬ ‭Group Norms:‬‭"Attitudinal and behavioural uniformities that define group membership &‬ ‭differentiate between groups."‬ ‭Key Studies on Group Norms:‬ ‭Siegel & Siegel (1957):‬‭Female students in liberal dormitories became less‬ ‭conservative over time, demonstrating the influence of group norms.‬ ‭Stormshak et al. (1999):‬‭Showed that classroom norms for aggression impacted the‬ ‭relationship between aggressive behaviour and peer preference in 1st graders.‬ ‭Deviating from group norms can lead to dislike and pressure to conform.‬ ‭4. Leadership‬ ‭This section defines leadership and explores different approaches to understanding effective‬ ‭leadership:‬ ‭Definition:‬‭"A process of social influence through which an individual enlists and‬ ‭mobilizes the aid of others in the attainment of a collective goal" (Chemers, 2001, p.‬ ‭376).‬ ‭Key Aspects: Relational, collective, purposeful.‬ ‭Approaches to Leadership:‬ ‭Personality:‬‭The "Great Person" approach links leadership to individual traits. Some‬ ‭correlation exists with higher intelligence, physique, talkativeness, and dominance, and‬ ‭with the Big Five personality dimensions.‬ ‭Leadership Styles (Lippitt & White, 1943):‬ ‭ utocratic:‬‭Leader makes decisions unilaterally, leading to high dependence and‬ A ‭potential de-motivation.‬ ‭Democratic:‬‭Encourages participation, fostering a positive atmosphere and high‬ ‭productivity.‬ ‭Laissez-faire:‬‭Hands-off approach, resulting in low productivity.‬ ‭Contingency Theories:‬‭Emphasise the interaction between leader type and situational‬ ‭control.‬ ‭Fiedler (1967, 1971, 1981):‬‭Proposed that task-oriented leaders thrive in high or low‬ ‭control situations, while relationship-oriented leaders excel in moderate control‬ ‭situations.‬ ‭Transactional Leadership:‬‭Focuses on the exchange of resources between leader and‬ ‭followers.‬ ‭Leader-Member Exchange (LMX) Theory:‬‭Highlights the importance of high-quality,‬ ‭personalised relationships for effective leadership.‬ ‭Transformational Leadership:‬‭Leaders inspire and motivate followers, transforming‬ ‭group goals and actions.‬ ‭Key Components (Avolio & Bass, 1987; Bass, 1985):‬‭Individualised consideration,‬ ‭intellectual stimulation, inspirational motivation, idealised influence (charisma).‬ ‭Cultural Influences on Leadership:‬ ‭Universally accepted/rejected traits:‬‭While some leadership traits are universally‬ ‭accepted (e.g., communication, trustworthiness) or rejected (e.g., being a loner,‬ ‭ruthless), cultural nuances exist.‬ ‭Leong & Fischer (2011):‬‭Found transformational leadership scores were higher in‬ ‭countries with higher mastery and egalitarianism values.‬ ‭This briefing document summarises key themes and findings related to social groups and‬ ‭leadership from the provided source. Understanding these concepts is crucial for‬ ‭comprehending group dynamics, individual behaviour within groups, and effective leadership‬ ‭strategies.‬ ‭Week 10: Intergroup Behaviour‬ I‭. Theoretical Explanations for Intergroup Behaviour‬ ‭A. Relative Deprivation:‬ ‭Definition:‬‭The perception that an individual or their group possesses less than they‬ ‭deserve compared to similar others or groups. This perception involves comparison, a‬ ‭sense of disadvantage, and a belief that the disadvantage is unfair.‬ ‭Types:‬ ‭Egoistic relative deprivation:‬‭Comparison with other individuals.‬ ‭Fraternalistic relative deprivation:‬‭Comparison with other groups.‬ ‭Link to Intergroup Aggression:‬‭Fraternalistic relative deprivation can predict support‬ ‭for and involvement in aggressive behaviours, especially when:‬ ‭The comparison group is clearly defined.‬ ‭There is a perceived injustice (distributive or procedural).‬ ‭There is a strong identification with the in-group.‬ ‭Collective action is deemed practical and feasible.‬ ‭B. Realistic Conflict Theory:‬ ‭Premise:‬‭Intergroup behaviour stems from the nature of goal relations between groups.‬ ‭Sherif's Robbers Cave Experiment:‬‭Demonstrated that competition over scarce‬ ‭resources leads to intergroup conflict and ethnocentrism. The experiment also showed‬ ‭that introducing superordinate goals can reduce intergroup hostility.‬ ‭Key Points:‬‭Shared goals foster intergroup cooperation and harmony.‬ ‭Mutually exclusive goals lead to intergroup competition, conflict, and ethnocentrism.‬ ‭C. Social Identity Approach:‬ ‭Minimal Group Paradigm:‬‭Tajfel's research demonstrated that mere categorization into‬ ‭groups, even based on arbitrary criteria, leads to in-group favouritism and out-group‬ ‭discrimination. This occurs even in the absence of self-interest, shared knowledge,‬ ‭proximity, or a shared future.‬ ‭Social Identity Theory (SIT):‬‭Focuses on intergroup behaviour and posits that‬ ‭individuals strive for a positive social identity by comparing their in-group favourably to‬ ‭relevant out-groups.‬ ‭Key Concepts:‬‭Categorization, comparison, and the pursuit of positive social identity.‬ ‭Self-Categorization Theory (SCT):‬‭Focuses on how individuals act as a group by‬ ‭categorizing themselves and others based on salient similarities and differences.‬ ‭Key Concepts:‬‭Self-categorization, the meta-contrast principle (minimizing intra-group‬ ‭differences and maximizing intergroup differences), and depersonalisation‬ ‭(self-stereotyping).‬ ‭Strategies for Improving Social Identity:‬ ‭Individual mobility:‬‭Changing group membership (feasible in flexible and permeable‬ ‭societies).‬ ‭Social creativity:‬‭Reframing the comparison or highlighting different dimensions.‬ ‭Social competition:‬‭Directly challenging the out-group to improve the in-group's status.‬ ‭D. Social Identity and Culture:‬ ‭Individualistic Cultures:‬‭Tend to focus on intergroup comparisons, show less in-group‬ ‭bias in real-life groups, and may disassociate from poorly performing in-groups.‬ ‭Collectivist Cultures:‬‭Tend to focus on intragroup relationships, exhibit greater in-group‬ ‭bias in real-life groups, and may derogate out-groups in response to poor in-group‬ ‭performance.‬ ‭Bicultural Identity:‬‭Individuals who identify with both their heritage culture and the‬ ‭dominant national culture tend to experience positive psychological and sociocultural‬ ‭adjustment.‬ ‭II. Collective Behaviour‬ ‭Definition:‬‭The behaviour of people in large groups (e.g., crowds, protests, riots).‬ ‭Theoretical Explanations:‬ ‭Le Bon's Theory:‬‭Viewed collective behaviour as pathological, driven by anonymity,‬ ‭contagion, and the emergence of primitive instincts.‬ ‭De-individuation:‬‭A loss of socialised individual identity, often leading to unsocialised‬ ‭behaviour. Factors contributing to de-individuation include group presence, anonymity,‬ ‭arousal, diffusion of responsibility, and reduced self-awareness. Research supports the‬ ‭link between anonymity and transgressive behaviour, even in online contexts.‬ ‭ mergent Norm Theory:‬‭Norms emerge from within the crowd to regulate behaviour.‬ E ‭However, this theory has been challenged.‬ ‭Social Identity Approach (SIDE):‬‭Collective behaviour is viewed as an intergroup‬ ‭phenomenon driven by a shift to social identity and conformity to context-specific norms.‬ ‭Research indicates that anonymity and social identification can contribute to negative‬ ‭collective behaviour like cyberbullying.‬ ‭III. Improving Intergroup Relations‬ ‭A. Intergroup Contact Hypothesis:‬ ‭Premise:‬‭Hostility stems from unfamiliarity and separation. Contact between groups,‬ ‭under optimal conditions, can improve intergroup relations.‬ ‭Optimal Conditions:‬‭Equal status between groups.‬ ‭Shared goals requiring intergroup cooperation.‬ ‭Social and institutional support for intergroup contact.‬ ‭Opportunities for repeated informal interaction and friendship potential.‬ ‭Mechanisms:‬‭Increased knowledge about the outgroup.‬ ‭Reduced intergroup anxiety.‬ ‭Enhanced empathy and perspective-taking.‬ ‭Meta-Analytic Evidence:‬‭Contact consistently reduces prejudice across diverse‬ ‭contexts and generalizes to various group memberships.‬ ‭B. Challenges for Intergroup Contact:‬ ‭Ensuring positive contact experiences.‬ ‭Achieving shared goals and avoiding outgroup blame for failures.‬ ‭Generalizing positive contact experiences beyond the immediate situation.‬ ‭Addressing the lack of contact opportunities.‬ ‭Overcoming social change and systemic issues that perpetuate inequalities.‬ ‭C. Strategies for Generalizing Contact Effects:‬ ‭Decategorization:‬‭Minimizing category salience to foster interpersonal interactions.‬ ‭Mutual Differentiation:‬‭Maintaining distinct group identities while highlighting positive‬ ‭intergroup interdependence.‬ ‭Common Ingroup Identity Model:‬‭Creating a superordinate identity encompassing‬ ‭both groups.‬ ‭Temporal Model:‬‭Progressing through stages of decategorization, differentiation, and‬ ‭common identity development.‬ ‭Integrative Theory:‬‭Combining salience of group memberships with interpersonal‬ ‭closeness and valuing each group's contribution.‬ ‭D. Expanding Contact Opportunities:‬ ‭Extended contact:‬‭Knowing ingroup members with outgroup friends.‬ ‭Vicarious contact:‬‭Observing positive intergroup interactions (e.g., media).‬ ‭Parasocial contact:‬‭Exposure to outgroup members through media.‬ ‭Imagined contact:‬‭Mentally simulating positive interactions.‬ ‭E-contact:‬‭Online intergroup cooperation.‬ ‭E. Integrating Contact and Collective Action (ICCA):‬ ‭This framework recognizes the importance of building trust and addressing inequalities.‬ I‭t suggests that contact interventions for disadvantaged groups should focus on‬ ‭empowering collective action while contact interventions for advantaged groups should‬ ‭emphasize allyship and support for social change.‬ ‭This briefing document highlights key insights into intergroup behaviour, offering a foundation for‬ ‭understanding the complexities of intergroup relations and identifying potential pathways‬ ‭towards more positive and harmonious intergroup dynamics.‬ ‭Week 11: Prejudice & Discrimination‬ ‭ hat is Prejudice?‬ W ‭Prejudice, meaning "judgement in advance," involves pre-judging individuals based solely on‬ ‭their group membership. While it can target any group, research primarily focuses on prejudice‬ ‭experienced by historically disadvantaged minorities.‬ ‭Components of Prejudice:‬ ‭Prejudice manifests through three interconnected components:‬ ‭Cognitive:‬‭Stereotypes:‬‭Stereotypes are overgeneralized beliefs about social groups,‬ ‭acting as schemas that influence information processing. They are pervasive, persistent,‬ ‭and can be both positive and negative. The Stereotype Content Model (Cuddy, Fiske &‬ ‭Glick, 2008) posits that stereotypes vary along two dimensions:‬ ‭Perceived warmth:‬‭Influenced by a group's perceived competitiveness.‬ ‭Perceived competence:‬‭Influenced by a group's perceived status.‬ ‭Affective: Emotions:‬‭Prejudice involves feelings toward a group, ranging from‬ ‭admiration to disgust. These emotions are often linked to stereotypes. For instance,‬ ‭high-competence, low-warmth groups might evoke envy.‬ ‭Behavioural: Discrimination:‬‭This component encompasses actions toward a group‬ ‭based on their membership. It can be direct (e.g., verbal abuse) or indirect (e.g.,‬ ‭avoidance), and can be individual or systemic.‬ ‭Current Forms and Frequency:‬ ‭While explicit prejudice is less common due to changing social norms, it has evolved into more‬ ‭subtle forms. Modern prejudice often involves denying the existence of prejudice and‬ ‭rationalizing inequalities while harboring implicit biases. Australian data highlights the‬ ‭persistence of prejudice:‬ ‭In 2018, a significant percentage of people reported high levels of prejudice towards‬ ‭various minority groups. (Faulkner, Zhao, & Smith, 2019)‬ ‭In 2020, a concerning number of people reported experiencing discrimination based on‬ ‭factors like disability, country of birth, and sexual orientation. (Australian Bureau of‬ ‭Statistics, 2020)‬ ‭Impact on Victims:‬ ‭Prejudice has profound negative consequences for victims, impacting well-being, performance,‬ ‭and perpetuating systemic inequalities.‬ ‭Well-being:‬‭Prejudice is linked to elevated stress, lower self-esteem, diminished life‬ ‭satisfaction, and poorer mental and physical health. A study on Aboriginal and Torres‬ ‭Strait Islander adults found that higher levels of discrimination were associated with‬ ‭poorer outcomes across various well-being indicators. (Thurber et al., 2021)‬ ‭Performance:‬‭Prejudice can undermine performance through:‬ ‭ tereotype threat:‬‭Concerns about confirming negative stereotypes impede‬ S ‭performance. Examples include women in mathematics, Black students in intellectual‬ ‭tasks, and older people in memory tests.‬ ‭Self-fulfilling prophecy:‬‭Expectations about someone influence interactions, leading to‬ ‭behavioral changes that confirm those expectations. A French study (Glover, Pallis &‬ ‭Pariente, 2017) demonstrated that minority workers were less productive under biased‬ ‭managers, highlighting discrimination as a self-fulfilling prophecy.‬ ‭Systemic Discrimination:‬‭A network of rules and practices that disadvantage less‬ ‭empowered groups while advantaging dominant groups. Examples include racial‬ ‭profiling by law enforcement, discriminatory hiring practices, and unequal access to‬ ‭healthcare. Research (Booth, Leigh & Varganova, 2012) using fake job applications with‬ ‭racially diverse names revealed higher call-back rates for Anglo-Saxon names,‬ ‭demonstrating systemic bias in hiring.‬ ‭Theories of Prejudice:‬ ‭Several theories attempt to explain the origins and persistence of prejudice:‬ ‭Individual Explanations: Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis:‬‭Frustration can lead to‬ ‭aggression, often displaced onto scapegoats. This explains some prejudiced behavior‬ ‭but struggles to account for its widespread uniformity.‬ ‭Authoritarian Personality & Right-Wing Authoritarianism:‬‭Certain personality traits,‬ ‭like rigid thinking and adherence to authority, are associated with prejudice. However,‬ ‭these approaches downplay situational factors.‬ ‭Interpersonal Explanations: Belief Congruence Theory:‬‭Dissimilarity in beliefs can‬ ‭lead to prejudice, but this theory fails to explain prejudice in situations where it is socially‬ ‭sanctioned.‬ ‭Group-Based Explanations: Realistic Conflict Theory:‬‭Competition over limited‬ ‭resources fuels intergroup conflict and prejudice.‬ ‭Integrated Threat Theory:‬‭Perceived threats from outgroups, whether realistic (e.g.,‬ ‭competition) or symbolic (e.g., differing values), contribute to prejudice. An Australian‬ ‭study (Schweitzer et al., 2005) found that both realistic and symbolic threats predicted‬ ‭prejudice towards refugees.‬ ‭Social Identity Perspective:‬‭Categorization into in-groups and out-groups leads to‬ ‭in-group bias, which can manifest as prejudice under specific conditions like high‬ ‭in-group identification and perceived out-group threat.‬ ‭Culture-Based Explanations:‬‭Cultural norms play a powerful role in shaping prejudice‬ ‭by dictating which groups are favored or denigrated.‬ ‭Reducing Prejudice:‬ ‭Various approaches aim to reduce prejudice, with research (Hsieh, Faulkner & Wickes, 2022)‬ ‭highlighting promising strategies:‬ ‭Awareness‬‭: Raising awareness about the negative consequences of prejudice and‬ ‭challenging stereotypical views.‬ ‭Contact:‬‭Promoting positive contact between groups, whether direct, extended, or‬ ‭virtual.‬ ‭Categorisation:‬‭Recategorizing in-group and out-group members under a shared‬ ‭superordinate category.‬ ‭ ocial Norms:‬‭Challenging prejudiced norms through positive role models and media‬ S ‭representations.‬ ‭Perspective-Taking:‬‭Encouraging empathy by highlighting shared experiences.‬ ‭Perceived Variability:‬‭Emphasizing the diverse characteristics within outgroups.‬ ‭Conclusion:‬ ‭Prejudice remains a significant social issue with detrimental consequences for individuals and‬ ‭society. Understanding its multifaceted nature, impact, and the theoretical explanations for its‬ ‭persistence are crucial for developing effective strategies to reduce prejudice and promote‬ ‭equality.‬ ‭Week 12: Prosocial Behaviour‬ ‭Defining Prosocial Behaviour:‬ ‭Prosocial behaviour encompasses acts that are positively valued by society. This broad‬ ‭category includes:‬ ‭Helping:‬‭Actions intentionally aimed at benefiting others.‬ ‭Altruism:‬‭A subset of helping where the act is intended to benefit others without any‬ ‭thought of personal gain.‬ ‭The distinction between helping and altruism hinges on motivation. Determining true altruistic‬ ‭motivation can be challenging.‬ ‭Cultural Influences:‬ ‭Cross-cultural research highlights the following trends:‬ ‭Prosocial behaviour is positively linked to values like benevolence and universalism,‬ ‭while negatively associated with achievement and power.‬ ‭Helping flourishes in cultures prioritizing self-transcendence over self-enhancement and‬ ‭conservatism over openness to change.‬ ‭Altruism is generally defined in terms of helping/doing good, encompassing sacrifice and‬ ‭selflessness.‬ ‭Explanations of Prosocial Behaviour:‬ ‭Several theoretical perspectives attempt to explain prosocial behaviour:‬ ‭Evolutionary Psychology:‬‭This approach posits that prosocial behaviours, at least in‬ ‭part, are driven by evolved biological factors aimed at maximizing an organism's genes‬ ‭in future generations.‬ ‭Kin selection‬‭suggests we're more likely to help relatives to ensure the survival and‬ ‭reproduction of shared genes. A study by Burnstein et al. (1994) found participants were‬ ‭more likely to help close relatives, especially healthy ones, in hypothetical life-or-death‬ ‭scenarios.‬ ‭Gene-culture co-evolution theory‬‭proposes that prosocial behaviours beneficial to‬ ‭group survival are passed down through generations. Societies encouraging‬ ‭self-sacrifice for the collective good are more likely to thrive. The norm of reciprocity,‬ ‭where helping others increases the likelihood of receiving help in return, exemplifies this‬ ‭concept.‬ ‭Social Learning:‬‭This perspective emphasizes the role of learning and socialization in‬ ‭shaping prosocial behaviour:‬ ‭ odelling:‬‭Observing prosocial behaviour in real life, on television, or in video games‬ M ‭can increase helping behaviour. Bryan & Test (1967) demonstrated this in a field study‬ ‭where motorists were more likely to help a woman with a flat tire if they had previously‬ ‭observed someone else being helped.‬ ‭Direct Instruction:‬‭Explicitly telling children to be helpful encourages prosocial‬ ‭behaviour.‬ ‭Operant Conditioning:‬‭Rewarding (reinforcement) helpful actions and punishing‬ ‭unhelpful ones shapes behaviour.‬ ‭Social Exchange Theory:‬‭This framework suggests that our actions, including helping,‬ ‭are motivated by a desire to maximize benefits and minimize costs. Benefits of helping‬ ‭can include social approval, reduced personal distress, and increased self-worth. Costs‬ ‭include risk, time, effort, and financial expenditure. We weigh these factors before‬ ‭deciding to help.‬ ‭Bystander-Calculus Model (Piliavin et al., 1981):‬‭This model explains helping‬ ‭behaviour in emergencies, proposing three stages:‬ ‭Physiological Arousal:‬‭Witnessing an emergency triggers arousal. Higher arousal‬ ‭increases the likelihood of helping.‬ ‭Labelling the Arousal:‬‭This arousal is interpreted as empathic concern, motivating‬ ‭helping.‬ ‭Evaluating the Consequences:‬‭The bystander weighs the costs of helping (e.g.,‬ ‭danger) against the costs of not helping (e.g., guilt). Helping is more likely when the‬ ‭costs of not helping are higher.‬ ‭Empathy-Altruism Hypothesis (Batson, 1991):‬‭This model suggests that observing‬ ‭others in distress triggers two possible responses:‬ ‭Empathy:‬‭The ability to understand and share another person's feelings. Empathy leads‬ ‭to altruistic helping, where the goal is to relieve the other person's suffering, regardless‬ ‭of personal costs or benefits.‬ ‭Personal Distress:‬‭Focus on one's own feelings of discomfort. This can lead to either‬ ‭inaction or egoistic helping, motivated by reducing one's own distress. Toi & Batson‬ ‭(1982) provided evidence for this hypothesis, finding that participants induced to feel‬ ‭high empathy were equally likely to help regardless of the personal cost, while those with‬ ‭low empathy helped more when the cost of not helping was high.‬ ‭Other Determinants of Helping:‬ ‭Perceiver Characteristics:‬ ‭Personality:‬‭While there's no universal "altruistic personality," certain traits like‬ ‭agreeableness, forgiveness, and need for approval are associated with prosocial‬ ‭behaviour.‬ ‭Mood:‬‭People in good moods are more likely to help. Bad moods can have mixed‬ ‭effects: self-concern might decrease helping, but guilt or the desire to alleviate negative‬ ‭feelings (negative-state relief model) can increase it.‬ ‭Competence:‬‭Possessing relevant skills increases the likelihood of helping.‬ ‭Gender:‬‭Evidence for gender differences in helping is limited. Socially prescribed roles‬ ‭may be more influential than gender itself.‬ ‭Receiver Characteristics:‬ ‭ imilarity:‬‭We are more likely to help those perceived as similar to ourselves.‬ S ‭Attractiveness:‬‭Attractive individuals are more likely to receive help.‬ ‭Responsibility for Misfortune:‬‭Help is more readily offered to those perceived as‬ ‭needing help due to circumstances beyond their control.‬ ‭Social Context:‬ ‭Urban vs. Rural:‬‭People in rural areas are generally more helpful than those in urban‬ ‭centres. This is attributed to the situational factors of urban environments, including‬ ‭increased anonymity, greater demands on resources, higher perceived risk, and the‬ ‭assumption that authorities will handle emergencies.‬ ‭Culture:‬‭Helping behaviour varies across cultures. Simpatia cultures (e.g., Latin‬ ‭America, Spain) characterized by concern for others' well-being, show higher levels of‬ ‭helping. Research by Levine et al. (2001) demonstrated this, with Rio de Janeiro and‬ ‭San Jose topping the list of helpful cities, while New York and Kuala Lumpur were‬ ‭among the least helpful. Conversely, cultures high in embeddedness (valuing social‬ ‭order and tradition) show lower levels of helping (Knafo et al., 2009).‬ ‭Helping in Emergencies:‬ ‭The tragic case of Kitty Genovese, who was murdered while numerous bystanders failed to‬ ‭intervene, highlighted the bystander effect:‬ ‭Bystander Effect:‬‭The probability of help being offered decreases as the number of‬ ‭bystanders increases.‬ ‭This phenomenon is attributed to several factors:‬ ‭Ambiguity:‬‭Emergencies are often ambiguous, making it unclear whether help is‬ ‭needed.‬ ‭Diffusion of Responsibility:‬‭With multiple bystanders, individuals feel less personal‬ ‭responsibility to act.‬ ‭Pluralistic Ignorance:‬‭Bystanders observing others' inaction may conclude that the‬ ‭situation is not an emergency.‬ ‭Audience Inhibition:‬‭Fear of social disapproval for intervening inappropriately can‬ ‭prevent action.‬ ‭The Cognitive Model (Latane & Darley, 1968):‬‭This model outlines the steps involved in‬ ‭deciding to help in an emergency:‬ ‭Notice/Attend to the Event:‬‭Barriers include cognitive load and distractions.‬ ‭Interpret/Define the Event as an Emergency:‬‭Ambiguity and pluralistic ignorance can‬ ‭hinder this stage.‬ ‭Assume Responsibility:‬‭Diffusion of responsibility is a key barrier.‬ ‭Decide What Can Be Done:‬‭Lack of competence can prevent action.‬ ‭Enact the Help:‬‭Costs outweighing rewards and audience inhibition can prevent‬ ‭helping.‬ ‭A meta-analysis by Fischer et al. (2011) confirmed the bystander effect but found it to be weaker‬ ‭in dangerous emergencies, possibly due to increased clarity and the need for cooperation.‬ ‭Encouraging Bystander Intervention:‬ ‭Strategies to increase helping in emergencies include:‬ ‭Teaching children to "disobey" and seek help in emergencies.‬ ‭Educating the public about the bystander effect.‬ ‭ romoting leadership roles and encouraging individuals to take charge.‬ P ‭Equipping people with the skills and knowledge to help, through first aid training and‬ ‭bystander intervention programs addressing issues like bullying and prejudice.‬

Use Quizgecko on...
Browser
Browser