Psychology Memory Notes PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by Deleted User
Tags
Summary
This document provides an overview of memory theories, focusing on the multi-store model and different types of long-term memory, such as episodic and semantic memory. It includes key features, strengths, and weaknesses of each model and relevant supporting research, demonstrating the different aspects that contribute to human memory.
Full Transcript
**[MEMORY ]** - The multi-store model of memory: sensory register, short-term memory and long-term memory. Features of each store: coding, capacity and duration - **The Multi-Store Model of Memory (MSM)** - Developed by **Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968)**, the Multi-Store Model...
**[MEMORY ]** - The multi-store model of memory: sensory register, short-term memory and long-term memory. Features of each store: coding, capacity and duration - **The Multi-Store Model of Memory (MSM)** - Developed by **Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968)**, the Multi-Store Model (MSM) describes memory as a linear process where information passes through three distinct stores: **Sensory Register**, **Short-Term Memory (STM)**, and **Long-Term Memory (LTM)**. The model highlights differences in **coding**, **capacity**, and **duration** across these stores. - - **1. Overview of MSM's Structure** - **Linear Flow**: Information moves sequentially from one store to the next. - **Attention and Rehearsal**: Essential processes for transferring information between stores. - **Unitary Stores**: Each store functions independently with unique features. - **2. Sensory Register (SR)** - The **Sensory Register** is the first stage in MSM, where sensory information from the environment briefly enters the memory system. - **Key Features of the Sensory Register** - **Coding**: - **Modality-Specific**: Coding is specific to the sensory input type: - **Iconic Memory**: Visual information. - **Echoic Memory**: Auditory information. - **Automatic**: No conscious control over this encoding. - **Capacity**: - **Very Large**: Can hold vast amounts of sensory data briefly. - **Supporting Research**: **Sperling (1960)** found that participants could remember more items in a visual grid when asked to recall specific rows, indicating large capacity. - **Duration**: - **Very Short**: Lasts only milliseconds to seconds: - **Iconic Memory**: Approximately 0.5 seconds. - **Echoic Memory**: 2--4 seconds. - **Sperling (1960)**: Showed that visual information decays rapidly unless attended to. - ***Evaluation of the Sensory Register** * - **Strengths**: - **Modality-Specific Encoding**: Supported by research indicating different memory pathways for visual and auditory information. - **Weaknesses**: - **Difficult to Study**: Due to the very brief duration, accurately measuring the Sensory Register is challenging. - - **3. Short-Term Memory (STM)** - Short-Term Memory (STM) is where attended information is processed briefly before being forgotten or transferred to LTM through rehearsal. - **Key Features of Short-Term Memory** - **Coding**: - **Primarily Acoustic**: Information is mainly encoded by sound. - **Conrad (1964)**: Similar-sounding letters (e.g., B, P, T) are more likely to be confused, indicating reliance on sound-based encoding. - **Capacity**: - **Limited**: 7 ± 2 items, as proposed by **Miller (1956)**. - **Chunking**: Grouping information (e.g., a phone number) can increase the amount STM can hold. - **Refined Understanding**: Some research suggests STM may hold closer to 4-5 items, especially for complex information. - **Duration**: - **Short (18--30 seconds)** without rehearsal. - **Peterson and Peterson (1959)**: Participants could not recall trigrams (nonsense syllables) accurately after 18 seconds without rehearsal. - ***Evaluation of Short-Term Memory** * - **Strengths**: - **Strong Empirical Support**: Studies by Miller (1956) and Peterson and Peterson (1959) back MSM's claims about STM's limited capacity and short duration. - **Practical Application in Learning**: **Chunking** strategies, based on Miller's findings, are used to improve memory retention. - **Weaknesses**: - **Oversimplified**: Research on **Working Memory Model (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974)** suggests STM has multiple subsystems, not a single unitary store. - **Artificiality of Lab Research**: Tasks like recalling trigrams lack real-world relevance, which may affect how accurately the findings reflect actual STM capacity and duration. - - **4. Long-Term Memory (LTM)** - Long-Term Memory (LTM) is the store for information that has been rehearsed sufficiently and can potentially be held indefinitely. - **Key Features of Long-Term Memory** - **Coding**: - **Primarily Semantic**: Information is mainly encoded by meaning. - **Baddeley (1966)**: Found that similar meanings (e.g., "large" and "big") create confusion, supporting semantic coding in LTM. - **Capacity**: - **Virtually Unlimited**: No known limits to how much LTM can store. - **Bahrick et al. (1975)**: Participants could recognize classmates in photos even 50 years later, suggesting LTM has a substantial capacity for meaningful information. - **Duration**: - **Potentially Lifelong**: Information in LTM can last decades or even a lifetime. - **Bahrick et al. (1975)**: Demonstrated LTM's enduring capacity in a study where participants remembered classmates' names and faces after decades. - **Rehearsal and Retrieval**: Regular recall strengthens memory retention. - ***Evaluation of Long-Term Memory** * - **Strengths**: - **Strong Research Evidence**: Studies like those of Baddeley (1966) and Bahrick (1975) provide solid support for LTM's capacity, duration, and reliance on semantic coding. - **Real-World Application**: Emphasizes semantic understanding for long-term retention, useful in educational contexts. - **Weaknesses**: - **Oversimplification of LTM**: **Tulving's Theory (1985)** suggests LTM has distinct types (e.g., episodic, semantic, procedural), indicating MSM's unitary view of LTM is too simplistic. - **Limitations of Rehearsal-Only View**: Some memories (like flashbulb memories) can be stored without rehearsal, challenging the MSM's emphasis on rehearsal for memory transfer. - - **5. Supporting Studies and Their Implications** - **Key Studies That Support MSM's Claims** - **Sperling (1960)** - **Sensory Register Capacity**: - Showed large but brief storage in the Sensory Register. - **Peterson and Peterson (1959)** - **STM Duration**: - Confirmed that STM holds information briefly unless rehearsed. - **Miller (1956)** - **STM Capacity**: - Found STM can hold 7 ± 2 items, which led to the concept of chunking. - **Baddeley (1966)** - **Coding Differences in STM and LTM**: - STM primarily uses acoustic encoding, while LTM relies on semantic encoding. - **Bahrick et al. (1975)** - **LTM Duration**: - Demonstrated that meaningful information can last decades in LTM. - - **6. Evaluation of the Multi-Store Model (MSM) Overall** - **Strengths of the MSM** - **Foundational Influence**: - MSM introduced the idea of separate memory stores, inspiring further memory research and models. - **Empirical Support**: - Studies on capacity, duration, and coding (like those from Sperling, Miller, and Baddeley) strongly support MSM's structure. - **Serial Position Effect**: - Research by **Glanzer and Cunitz (1966)** found people remember words at the start (primacy) and end (recency) of lists better, suggesting separate STM and LTM stores. - **Weaknesses of the MSM** - **Oversimplification**: - STM and LTM aren't as simple as MSM suggests. The **Working Memory Model** and **Tulving's Types of LTM** show memory is more complex, with STM and LTM having multiple subsystems. - **Rehearsal Overemphasized**: - MSM implies rehearsal is essential to transfer information to LTM, but **flashbulb memories** show emotional significance can lead to long-term storage without rehearsal. - **Artificiality of Supporting Studies**: - Many studies supporting MSM (like word lists) lack ecological validity, meaning they may not apply to real-life memory processes. -. - Types of long-term memory: episodic, semantic, procedural. **Types of Long-Term Memory (LTM)** **Endel Tulving (1985)** proposed that **Long-Term Memory (LTM)** is not a single, unified store but consists of different types of memory stores with distinct functions: **Episodic**, **Semantic**, and **Procedural Memory**. Each type of LTM has unique characteristics, involving different types of encoding, storage, and retrieval processes, and often different areas of the brain. **1. Episodic Memory** Episodic Memory refers to memories of personal experiences and specific events that have occurred in one's life, including contextual details like time, place, and associated emotions. ***Key Features of Episodic Memory** * - **Content**: Stores information about **events and experiences** (e.g., a birthday party, first day of school). - **Time-Stamped**: Memories are **time-stamped**, meaning they are linked to specific points in time and can be recalled in a sequence. - **Conscious Recall**: **Explicit** (declarative) memory that requires **conscious effort** to recall details. - **Emotionally Charged**: Often involves **emotional experiences**, which may strengthen recall. ***Supporting Research and Brain Areas** * - **Brain Region**: Associated with the **hippocampus** and **frontal lobe**. These areas are active when episodic memories are formed or recalled. - **Case Study - HM (Henry Molaison)**: - After surgery that removed parts of his hippocampus, HM lost the ability to form new episodic memories but retained procedural memory, suggesting episodic memory depends on the hippocampus. - **Case Study - Clive Wearing**: - A virus damaged his hippocampus, leading to severe episodic memory loss. He could not recall personal experiences but retained procedural memory (e.g., playing piano), supporting the separation of episodic and procedural memory. ***Evaluation of Episodic Memory** * - **Strengths**: - **Research Support**: Cases like HM and Clive Wearing show how damage to specific brain regions impairs episodic memory without affecting other types, supporting a distinct memory type. - **Practical Applications**: Understanding episodic memory is useful in treating amnesia and age-related memory loss. - **Limitations**: - **Challenges in Measuring**: Episodic memory is subjective, and variations in memory accuracy make it challenging to measure objectively. - **Overlap with Semantic Memory**: There is often overlap, as semantic knowledge (e.g., facts) can sometimes have episodic elements (e.g., learning how you found out a fact). **2. Semantic Memory** Semantic Memory stores general knowledge, facts, and concepts about the world. Unlike episodic memory, semantic memory is not tied to personal experience or a specific time and place. ***Key Features of Semantic Memory** * - **Content**: Includes **facts, knowledge, and concepts** (e.g., knowing that Paris is the capital of France, definitions, historical dates). - **Not Time-Stamped**: Semantic memories are **not linked to specific events** or personal experiences, making them **timeless**. - **Conscious Recall**: Like episodic memory, semantic memory is **explicit** and requires conscious effort for recall. ***Supporting Research and Brain Areas** * - **Brain Region**: Mainly associated with the **temporal lobe**, particularly the left side. - **Case Study - KC (Kent Cochrane)**: - KC suffered damage to his episodic memory while retaining most semantic memory, showing he could still recall facts without remembering personal experiences. - **HM**: Although unable to form new episodic memories, HM retained some semantic knowledge, which supports the idea of separate memory systems. ***Evaluation of Semantic Memory** * - **Strengths**: - **Supported by Case Studies**: Cases like KC and HM highlight individuals who retained semantic memory despite loss of episodic memory, showing they are distinct. - **Educational Applications**: Knowledge of semantic memory has applications in teaching and learning strategies that strengthen factual knowledge. - **Limitations**: - **Overlap with Episodic Memory**: Semantic knowledge is sometimes formed through repeated episodic experiences (e.g., learning facts in school), making it challenging to completely separate them. - **Individual Differences**: The structure of semantic memory varies widely among individuals, influenced by factors like education and experience, complicating generalization. **3. Procedural Memory** Procedural Memory is the store for knowledge of skills and actions, often referred to as "muscle memory" or memory for "how to" perform tasks. ***Key Features of Procedural Memory** * - **Content**: Includes knowledge of **skills and actions** (e.g., riding a bike, typing, playing a musical instrument). - **Implicit Recall**: **Non-declarative** memory; it operates **unconsciously** and does not require active thinking or recollection. - **Automaticity**: Over time, procedural memory becomes **automatic**, requiring little conscious thought, which allows for multitasking (e.g., driving while talking). ***Supporting Research and Brain Areas** * - **Brain Regions**: Procedural memory involves the **cerebellum** and **basal ganglia**, which are essential for motor skills and coordination. - **Case Study - Clive Wearing**: - Despite losing episodic memory, Clive Wearing retained his ability to play the piano, demonstrating that procedural memory operates independently of episodic memory. - **HM**: HM could learn new procedural tasks, such as drawing while looking in a mirror, showing procedural memory formation even without new episodic memories. ***Evaluation of Procedural Memory** * - **Strengths**: - **Research Support**: Studies on amnesic patients (like Clive Wearing and HM) who retain procedural skills despite episodic/semantic impairments confirm the distinct nature of procedural memory. - **Application in Skill Learning**: Knowing about procedural memory aids in developing techniques to improve skill learning and rehabilitation. - **Limitations**: - **Difficulty in Verbalization**: Procedural memory is hard to explain or "declare," making it challenging to study directly. - **Overlap with Motor Skills**: Procedural memory heavily overlaps with motor skill areas, making it less distinct in function than episodic and semantic memory. **Summary of Types of LTM and Key Differences** **Type of LTM** **Content** **Time-Stamped?** **Conscious or Automatic?** **Brain Areas Involved** ------------------------ ---------------------------------- -------------------- ------------------------------ ---------------------------- **Episodic Memory** Events and personal experiences Yes Conscious (explicit) Hippocampus, Frontal Lobe **Semantic Memory** Facts, concepts, and knowledge No Conscious (explicit) Temporal Lobe **Procedural Memory** Skills and actions No Automatic (implicit) Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia **Evaluation of Tulving's Types of LTM Theory (Overall)** **Strengths of the Types of LTM Theory** 1. **Research and Neuroimaging Support**: a. **Brain Scan Evidence**: PET scans have shown that different brain areas are active when using episodic versus semantic memories, supporting separate types of LTM. b. **Support from Case Studies**: Cases like HM, Clive Wearing, and KC provide real-world examples of memory functioning as separate systems. 2. **Practical Applications**: a. **Memory and Learning Interventions**: Knowing the distinctions between memory types has applications in education, cognitive therapy, and treatments for memory loss. b. **Skill Training**: Understanding procedural memory helps in designing effective training programs for motor skills. **Limitations of the Types of LTM Theory** 1. **Potential Overlap Between Memory Types**: a. Many episodic memories contain semantic elements (e.g., learning a fact through personal experience), making a clear distinction difficult. 2. **Limited Generalizability**: a. Case studies provide deep insights but involve unique brain injuries, so findings may not fully apply to the general population. 3. **Alternative Theories**: a. Other theories propose more complex views of LTM, such as **Squire and Zola's declarative/non-declarative memory model**, which suggests additional layers beyond episodic and semantic divisions. - The working memory model: central executive, phonological loop, visuo-spatial sketchpad and episodic buffer. Features of the model: coding and capacity. **The Working Memory Model (WMM)** **Developed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974)**, the Working Memory Model (WMM) is a theory of Short-Term Memory (STM) that describes it as a complex system with multiple components, rather than a single, unitary store as suggested by the Multi-Store Model (MSM). It explains how we temporarily hold and manipulate information for complex cognitive tasks like reasoning, learning, and comprehension. **1. Overview of the WMM's Structure** - **Multiple Components**: Unlike the MSM's single STM store, WMM proposes that STM has different components to handle different types of information. - **Focus on Active Processing**: WMM emphasizes the **active manipulation** of information, rather than just storage. - **Components**: - **Central Executive** - **Phonological Loop** - **Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad** - **Episodic Buffer** **2. The Central Executive (CE)** The **Central Executive (CE)** is the "control center" of the Working Memory Model, responsible for directing attention, coordinating the activities of the other components, and allocating resources. **Key Features of the Central Executive** - **Coding**: Modality-free, meaning it processes information from any sensory modality (e.g., visual, auditory). - **Capacity**: Very limited, often described as a "bottleneck" that prevents overload. - Can only handle a limited amount of information at once. - Engages in selective attention to prioritize tasks. ***Supporting Research and Brain Areas** * - **Brain Region**: Linked to the **prefrontal cortex**, associated with executive functions like attention control and decision-making. - **Study - Baddeley (1996)**: - Found that participants struggled to perform tasks requiring attention to two activities simultaneously, supporting the idea of the CE's limited capacity. ***Evaluation of the Central Executive** * - **Strengths**: - **Experimental Support**: Studies show that people struggle to handle multiple demanding tasks, supporting the CE's role in resource allocation. - **Real-World Relevance**: Explains difficulties in multitasking, which has applications in fields like cognitive training and workload management. - **Limitations**: - **Vague Definition**: Critics argue that the CE is poorly defined and difficult to measure directly. - **Lack of Clarity on Capacity**: Little empirical evidence specifies exactly how much information it can handle, leaving the model somewhat ambiguous. **3. Phonological Loop (PL)** The **Phonological Loop (PL)** is responsible for processing and holding verbal and auditory information. It's essential for tasks involving language, such as reading or remembering spoken instructions. **Key Features of the Phonological Loop** - **Coding**: Acoustic (sound-based) coding, suitable for processing verbal and auditory information. - **Capacity**: Limited, generally holding information for 1.5 to 2 seconds unless rehearsed. ***Subcomponents of the Phonological Loop** * - **Phonological Store (Inner Ear)**: - Temporarily stores verbal information in a speech-based form. - Holds information passively for a short duration. - **Articulatory Control System (Inner Voice)**: - Engages in subvocal rehearsal to maintain information within the loop. - Repeats information to prevent decay, allowing for extended storage. ***Supporting Research** * - **Study - Baddeley et al. (1975) - Word Length Effect**: - Participants could recall shorter words more accurately than longer words, supporting the idea that the PL has a limited capacity. - **Study - Conrad (1964)**: - Found that similar-sounding letters were more likely to be confused, supporting acoustic coding in the PL. ***Evaluation of the Phonological Loop** * - **Strengths**: - **Strong Empirical Support**: Studies on the word length effect and sound confusion provide strong evidence for the PL's characteristics. - **Application in Language Learning**: Helps explain language acquisition and aids in designing effective language learning techniques. - **Limitations**: - **Does Not Explain All Auditory Processing**: The PL cannot account for non-verbal sounds (e.g., instrumental music), limiting its scope. - **Oversimplified**: Research on complex verbal processing suggests the PL might have more than just two subcomponents. **4. Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad (VSS)** The **Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad (VSS)** is responsible for processing visual and spatial information, playing a key role in tasks involving visual imagery, navigation, and spatial reasoning. **Key Features of the Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad** - **Coding**: Visual and spatial coding; processes information about what things look like and where they are located. - **Capacity**: Limited, typically holding 3--4 items at a time for effective use. ***Subcomponents of the Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad** * - **Visual Cache**: - Temporarily stores visual data (e.g., colors, shapes). - **Inner Scribe**: - Records the arrangement of objects in the visual field and manages spatial relationships. ***Supporting Research** * - **Brain Region**: Primarily associated with the **occipital lobe** (for visual processing) and **parietal lobe** (for spatial awareness). - **Study - Baddeley et al. (1975)**: - Found that participants struggled to perform two visual tasks simultaneously (e.g., tracking a light while describing a shape), supporting the VSS's limited capacity and its focus on visual-spatial information. ***Evaluation of the Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad** * - **Strengths**: - **Support for Visual-Spatial Distinctions**: Empirical support for the VSS's subcomponents (visual cache and inner scribe) enhances understanding of visual-spatial memory. - **Useful in Understanding Visual Learning**: Applies to educational contexts and visual learning strategies, such as using diagrams. - **Limitations**: - **Limited Research on the Inner Scribe**: Difficult to measure separately from the visual cache, leading to ambiguity. - **Overlooks Certain Visual Processing Aspects**: The model does not explain how we process complex visual tasks like face recognition, which may involve other brain areas. **5. Episodic Buffer (EB)** The **Episodic Buffer (EB)**, added by Baddeley in 2000, serves as a **temporary store** that integrates information from the other components (PL, VSS, and LTM) into a single, coherent memory trace. It acts as a bridge between working memory and long-term memory. **Key Features of the Episodic Buffer** - **Coding**: Multimodal (handles both visual and acoustic information) to create unified representations. - **Capacity**: Limited, typically around **4 chunks** of information at any one time. ***Role and Function** * - Integrates information from multiple sources (PL, VSS, LTM) to form a cohesive memory episode. - Facilitates conscious awareness by linking visual, spatial, and verbal information into a coherent whole, which can then be stored in LTM. ***Supporting Research** * - **Brain Region**: Evidence suggests it involves the **prefrontal cortex** but is more difficult to localize precisely. - **Study - Prabhakaran et al. (2000)**: - Used fMRI scans and found that certain brain areas were active during tasks requiring both visual and verbal information, suggesting that the EB integrates multimodal data. ***Evaluation of the Episodic Buffer** * - **Strengths**: - **Enhanced Explanatory Power**: By integrating multimodal information, the EB addresses limitations of the initial WMM, explaining how we form complex, episodic memories. - **Support for Multitasking**: Explains how we can perform tasks that require simultaneous processing from multiple modalities (e.g., following a map while listening to directions). - **Limitations**: - **Limited Empirical Support**: The EB is the most recently added component and has less empirical evidence than other components. - **Ambiguity in Function**: The exact mechanisms and role of the EB are not well-defined, making it harder to study or measure directly. **Summary of Working Memory Model Components** **Component** **Function** **Coding** **Capacity** **Brain Area(s)** ------------------------------ ------------------------------------------ ----------------- ------------------ ----------------------------- **Central Executive** Controls attention, allocates resources Modality-Free Very Limited Prefrontal Cortex **Phonological Loop** Processes verbal and auditory info Acoustic \~1.5-2 seconds Left Temporal Lobe **Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad** Processes visual and spatial info Visual/Spatial \~3-4 items Occipital & Parietal Lobes **Episodic Buffer** Integrates info into episodes Multimodal \~4 chunks Prefrontal Cortex **Evaluation of the Working Memory Model (WMM) Overall** **Strengths of the WMM** 1. **Empirical Support from Dual-Task Studies**: a. Research consistently supports the WMM's notion of separate subsystems (e.g., visual and auditory), which the MSM fails to account for. 2. **Application in Education and Training**: a. Useful in designing learning techniques and understanding multitasking limitations. 3. **Flexible and Adaptive**: a. Recognizes STM as a dynamic workspace, better fitting real-life cognitive demands than a single-store model. **Limitations of the WMM** 1. **Lack of Detail on Central Executive**: a. Critics argue that the CE is oversimplified and lacks empirical definition. 2. **Challenges in Measuring Components**: a. Some subsystems, like the Episodic Buffer, are difficult to isolate in experiments, limiting empirical support. 3. **Overlooks LTM Influence**: a. The WMM doesn't explain how LTM affects working memory in detail, though some influence is likely. - Explanations for forgetting: proactive and retroactive interference and retrieval failure due to absence of cues. **Explanations for Forgetting: Proactive & Retroactive Interference & Retrieval Failure** Forgetting is a natural process in memory, and several theories have been proposed to explain why information is sometimes lost or becomes inaccessible. The **two main theories** we will explore are: - **Interference Theory**: Proactive and retroactive interference. - **Retrieval Failure Theory**: Forgetting occurs due to the absence of cues. **1. Interference Theory** Interference Theory suggests that forgetting occurs when information in memory disrupts the ability to retrieve other information. This is often due to **competition** between memories stored in LTM. Interference can be **proactive** or **retroactive**. ***Proactive Interference (PI)** * **Proactive Interference** occurs when **old memories interfere with the retrieval of new memories**. Essentially, previously learned information makes it harder to learn or recall new information. **Key Features of Proactive Interference** - **Definition**: When older memories disrupt the ability to recall more recent information. - **Example**: If you change your phone number, you might still recall your old phone number when trying to dial the new one. - **Effect on Memory**: Proactive interference generally **slows down or prevents learning** of new material because old material interferes with the encoding of new material. **Evidence for Proactive Interference** - **Study - Underwood (1957)**: - Found that participants who had learned multiple lists of words remembered fewer words from the last list than those who learned fewer lists, demonstrating that earlier learned material interfered with the ability to recall more recent information. - **Study - Keppel & Underwood (1962)**: - Participants who learned a list of consonant syllables over multiple trials showed a decrease in recall accuracy as trials progressed. The early trials had proactive interference, which interfered with learning on later trials. **Evaluation of Proactive Interference** - **Strengths**: - **Experimental Evidence**: Studies like Underwood (1957) provide strong support for proactive interference. - **Real-Life Relevance**: Proactive interference is frequently observed in everyday situations (e.g., difficulty remembering a new password after years of using an old one). - **Limitations**: - **Individual Differences**: Not everyone experiences proactive interference in the same way, suggesting that personal factors (e.g., cognitive ability) may influence susceptibility. - **Overemphasis on Lab Studies**: Many studies supporting proactive interference, like those of Underwood, used artificial tasks that may not fully reflect natural memory processes. ***Retroactive Interference (RI)** * **Retroactive Interference** occurs when **new memories interfere with the retrieval of older memories**. In this case, recently learned information makes it harder to recall earlier learned information. **Key Features of Retroactive Interference** - **Definition**: When new learning disrupts the recall of previously learned information. - **Example**: After learning a new phone number, you might forget your old one. - **Effect on Memory**: Retroactive interference tends to affect the **recall of old material** rather than the encoding of new material. **Evidence for Retroactive Interference** - **Study - Muller & Pilzecker (1900)**: - Participants who learned a list of nonsense syllables and were then given a delay before being asked to recall them performed worse than those who immediately recalled after learning. The delay allowed new information to interfere with memory retention. - **Study - Schmidt et al. (2000)**: - Conducted a field study with people who had moved away from their childhood neighborhood. They found that people with more street names to remember had poorer recall of old street names, suggesting retroactive interference from learning new street names. **Evaluation of Retroactive Interference** - **Strengths**: - **Real-World Application**: Retroactive interference has practical implications for memory retention, such as difficulty remembering old information after learning something new. - **Support from Field Studies**: Schmidt et al. (2000) show that retroactive interference can occur in real-world situations, increasing the ecological validity of the findings. - **Limitations**: - **Limited Control in Field Studies**: Field studies like Schmidt's cannot control for all variables (e.g., individual differences in memory), reducing internal validity. - **Over-Simplified Explanation**: Both proactive and retroactive interference may oversimplify how forgetting occurs, not accounting for other factors such as encoding failure or retrieval failure. **2. Retrieval Failure Due to Absence of Cues** The **Retrieval Failure Theory** suggests that forgetting occurs when **memory retrieval fails** because of the absence of appropriate **cues**. When information is stored in long-term memory, it is encoded with **contextual cues** that assist with retrieval. If these cues are absent or unavailable during recall, we may struggle to retrieve the information. ***Key Features of Retrieval Failure** * - **Definition**: Forgetting occurs because the right cues are unavailable, preventing access to stored memories. - **Cue-Dependent Forgetting**: The idea that memories are easier to retrieve when we have the same cues that were present during encoding. - **Example**: If you study for an exam in a particular room, you may find it easier to recall the information when you\'re in that same room rather than a different environment. ***Types of Retrieval Cues** * 1. **Context-Dependent Cues**: External cues such as the physical environment. a. **Example**: A person may recall information better if they are in the same room in which they studied. 2. **State-Dependent Cues**: Internal cues such as emotional or physiological state. a. **Example**: Someone who learns something while happy may recall it better when they are in a happy state. 3. **Encoding Specificity Principle**: The theory that memory retrieval is most effective when the cues available at retrieval are **exactly the same** as those present at the time of encoding. ***Evidence for Retrieval Failure** * - **Study - Godden & Baddeley (1975)**: - **Context-Dependent Forgetting**: Divers who learned words underwater performed better when tested underwater, while those who learned on land performed better when tested on land. This shows that context (environment) plays a role in retrieval. - **Study - Carter & Cassaday (1998)**: - **State-Dependent Forgetting**: Participants who learned information under the influence of antihistamines (which caused drowsiness) recalled the information better when in the same drugged state. This demonstrated the role of internal cues in memory retrieval. ***Evaluation of Retrieval Failure** * - **Strengths**: - **Support from Real-World Studies**: Studies like Godden & Baddeley (1975) and Carter & Cassaday (1998) show that context and state-dependent cues can significantly influence recall, lending strong support to the retrieval failure theory. - **Wide Applicability**: Retrieval failure can explain a wide range of real-life forgetting, such as forgetting names at a party or forgetting something you learned when you are not in the same mental or environmental state as when you learned it. - **Limitations**: - **Lack of Ecological Validity**: Lab experiments (such as those by Godden & Baddeley) often use artificial tasks that may not fully reflect real-world memory processes, limiting the generalizability of the findings. - **Over-reliance on Recall**: Many studies focus on recall, but retrieval failure may also occur in recognition tasks. This suggests that retrieval failure may not account for all forms of forgetting. **Summary of Explanations for Forgetting** **Explanation** **Description** **Key Studies** **Strengths** **Limitations** ------------------------------------------ ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ ----------------------------------------------------- -------------------------------------------------------------------------------- ----------------------------------------------------------------------------- **Proactive Interference (PI)** Old information interferes with the recall of new information. Underwood (1957), Keppel & Underwood (1962) Supported by experimental studies, real-world applications. Individual differences, oversimplified model of forgetting. **Retroactive Interference (RI)** New information interferes with the recall of old information. Muller & Pilzecker (1900), Schmidt et al. (2000) Field study support, real-world relevance. Limited control in field studies, oversimplification of forgetting causes. **Retrieval Failure (Absence of Cues)** Forgetting occurs when retrieval cues are not available or are ineffective. Godden & Baddeley (1975), Carter & Cassaday (1998) Real-world applications, strong empirical support from lab and field studies. Lack of ecological validity in lab experiments, over-focus on recall. - Factors affecting the accuracy of eyewitness testimony: misleading information, including leading questions and post-event discussion; anxiety. **Factors Affecting the Accuracy of Eyewitness Testimony (EWT)** Eyewitness testimony is the account a person gives in the aftermath of an event that they have witnessed. However, various **factors** can affect the **accuracy** of eyewitness memory, leading to errors in recall or recognition. These include **misleading information** and **anxiety**. **1. Misleading Information** Misleading information refers to **incorrect or false details** that may be introduced to an eyewitness, influencing their recall of the event. This can happen through **leading questions** and **post-event discussion**. ***Leading Questions** * A **leading question** is one that suggests a particular answer. These questions can alter the way eyewitnesses remember an event, leading them to recall information that may not be true. **Key Features of Leading Questions** - **Definition**: A question that, because of its phrasing, encourages or \"leads\" the witness to provide a specific answer. - **Example**: "Was the man wearing a red shirt when he hit the victim?" (The wording suggests the man was wearing a red shirt, even if he wasn't). - **Effect on Memory**: Leading questions can influence the witness's **memory retrieval**, causing them to remember details they didn't actually see or to misinterpret what they witnessed. **Evidence for Leading Questions** - **Study - Loftus & Palmer (1974)**: - Participants watched a video of a car crash and were asked about the speed of the cars using different verbs in the question, e.g., "smashed," "collided," or "contacted." Those who were asked using the verb "smashed" reported higher speeds than those asked using "contacted." This shows that leading questions can alter memory recall. - **Study - Loftus (1975)**: - Participants were shown a car accident and later asked misleading questions about the accident. When asked whether there was broken glass (which was not present), those exposed to the misleading question were more likely to say there had been broken glass, suggesting that leading questions can change memory details. **Evaluation of Leading Questions** - **Strengths**: - **Controlled Experiment**: Loftus & Palmer (1974) conducted a highly controlled experiment, increasing the internal validity of their findings. - **Real-World Applications**: The findings have practical implications for the legal system, where leading questions could influence court proceedings. - **Limitations**: - **Artificial Tasks**: The studies used artificial tasks (watching videos of accidents), which may not fully represent real-life eyewitness situations. - **Individual Differences**: Some witnesses may be more suggestible to leading questions than others, and this can vary depending on factors such as age or cognitive ability. ***Post-Event Discussion** * Post-event discussion occurs when witnesses talk about the event with others after it has happened. This can result in **memory contamination**, where the memories of one person influence or distort the memories of others. **Key Features of Post-Event Discussion** - **Definition**: The conversation that witnesses may have with other people after witnessing an event, which can affect the accuracy of their memories. - **Example**: Two witnesses may discuss the details of a robbery, and one might unknowingly suggest that the thief wore a blue jacket, leading the other witness to falsely remember the jacket as blue. - **Effect on Memory**: This can lead to **memory conformity** (witnesses may change their memories to match what others recall) or **memory contamination** (details become distorted due to new information introduced in the discussion). **Evidence for Post-Event Discussion** - **Study - Gabbert et al. (2003)**: - Participants watched a video of a crime from different angles, so that each participant saw different details. Afterward, they were allowed to discuss the event with each other. Those who discussed the event were more likely to recall details that were not part of their original view (e.g., recalling details they hadn't seen). This demonstrates how post-event discussion can distort memory. **Evaluation of Post-Event Discussion** - **Strengths**: - **Field Experiment**: Gabbert et al. (2003) used a field experiment with naturalistic scenarios, making it more ecologically valid. - **Implications for the Legal System**: The study highlights the potential risks of witness collaboration in real-life investigations, emphasizing the importance of careful questioning. - **Limitations**: - **Lack of Control**: As with all field experiments, Gabbert's study lacked the tight control of a lab experiment, so there may be other variables influencing memory. - **Artificial Task**: Witnesses in studies like Gabbert's may not be as emotionally or psychologically invested in the task as real-life witnesses would be. **2. Anxiety and Its Effect on Eyewitness Testimony** Anxiety can have a significant impact on memory recall. High levels of anxiety may cause people to either forget important details or be more focused on their emotional reaction to the event than on peripheral details. ***Key Features of Anxiety** * - **Definition**: A state of heightened emotional arousal that may be caused by fear, stress, or anxiety-provoking events. - **Example**: An eyewitness may struggle to recall the features of a criminal because they were distracted by their own fear during the crime. - **Effect on Memory**: The **Yerkes-Dodson Law** suggests that memory performance follows an **inverted U-shaped curve** with anxiety. A moderate level of anxiety can improve memory recall, while too much or too little anxiety impairs recall. ***Evidence for Anxiety's Effect on Eyewitness Testimony** * - **Study - Loftus et al. (1987)**: - Participants were either exposed to a **high-anxiety condition** (e.g., a man emerging from a room holding a knife covered in blood) or a **low-anxiety condition** (e.g., a man holding a pen). In a subsequent identification task, participants who had seen the high-anxiety condition were less accurate in identifying the man, suggesting that anxiety negatively impacts eyewitness accuracy. - **Study - Johnson & Scott (1976)**: - Similar to Loftus et al. (1987), this study found that participants who saw an argument accompanied by a violent act (e.g., a man holding a bloody knife) had poorer recall of the perpetrator's face than those who witnessed a more neutral event. This supports the idea that anxiety during a crime reduces the accuracy of EWT. - **Study - Yuille & Cutshall (1986)**: - This study provided evidence that **high anxiety** may not always impair memory. It involved witnesses of a real-life shooting. Those who reported the highest levels of anxiety at the time of the event had the most accurate recall of the event, suggesting that real-life anxiety might have a different effect than lab-induced anxiety. **Evaluation of Anxiety's Effect on Eyewitness Testimony** - **Strengths**: - **Real-Life Application**: Yuille & Cutshall's (1986) study suggests that high anxiety can sometimes enhance eyewitness testimony in naturalistic settings, which contrasts with lab-based findings. - **Practical Relevance**: Findings about anxiety can help police and legal professionals assess the reliability of eyewitness testimonies and the way they question witnesses. - **Limitations**: - **Contradictory Findings**: The contrasting results from Loftus et al. (1987) and Yuille & Cutshall (1986) suggest that the relationship between anxiety and eyewitness accuracy is complex and context-dependent. - **Individual Differences**: People react to anxiety in different ways, so the effect of anxiety on memory could vary between individuals, complicating general conclusions. **Summary of Factors Affecting EWT** **Factor** **Description** **Key Studies** **Strengths** **Limitations** ---------------------------------------------------- -------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- ------------------------------------------------------------------------- ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------ ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- **Misleading Information: Leading Questions** Questions that suggest a particular answer, altering the accuracy of the eyewitness's recall. Loftus & Palmer (1974), Loftus (1975) Strong experimental support, real-world relevance. Artificial tasks, individual differences in susceptibility to leading questions. **Misleading Information: Post-Event Discussion** Conversations after the event that can distort or contaminate memories. Gabbert et al. (2003) Field study support, practical implications for legal settings. Lack of control in field studies, artificial task. **Anxiety** Emotional arousal can either impair or enhance memory recall depending on the level of anxiety. Loftus et al. (1987), Johnson & Scott (1976), Yuille & Cutshall (1986) Real-life application, contrasting findings provide a more nuanced understanding. Contradictory findings on anxiety's impact on memory, individual differences in reaction to anxiety. - Improving the accuracy of eyewitness testimony, including the use of the cognitive interview **Improving the Accuracy of Eyewitness Testimony: The Cognitive Interview** Eyewitness testimony (EWT) is crucial in the criminal justice system, but as we've discussed, it can be unreliable due to various factors. To improve the accuracy of EWT, **cognitive techniques** have been developed, one of the most well-known being the **Cognitive Interview (CI)**. The Cognitive Interview aims to enhance memory retrieval through specific techniques that reduce the chances of memory distortion and encourage more accurate recall. **1. The Cognitive Interview (CI)** The **Cognitive Interview** was developed by **Fisher and Geiselman (1992)** as an alternative to traditional police interviewing techniques. It incorporates a variety of psychological principles aimed at increasing the amount of accurate information provided by witnesses while minimizing the potential for false memories or suggestibility. The Cognitive Interview is based on the premise that **memory retrieval is complex**, and it can be influenced by factors like the way questions are asked, the environment, and the mental state of the witness. By using more effective interview techniques, the aim is to improve the accuracy of eyewitness testimony. ***Key Features of the Cognitive Interview** * There are four main techniques involved in the Cognitive Interview. These techniques aim to improve memory retrieval by altering the context and the way the witness is encouraged to recall information. **1. Context Reinstatement** - **Definition**: The witness is encouraged to mentally revisit the context of the event (e.g., the environment, emotional state, sensory details) to help trigger memory retrieval. - **How it works**: This technique works by helping the witness to recreate the scene in their mind, thereby providing additional retrieval cues that may assist in recalling more accurate details. - **Example**: A witness may be asked to recall the weather, what they were feeling, or what they could see around them at the time of the event. **2. Report Everything** - **Definition**: The witness is asked to recall all the details they can, regardless of how trivial or irrelevant they might seem. - **How it works**: Even seemingly minor details can be important, as they may form connections with other memories or help to reconstruct the event more accurately. - **Example**: A witness may recall hearing a car in the background, which may seem irrelevant, but it might help the investigator piece together the timeline of events. **3. Change Perspective** - **Definition**: The witness is asked to recall the event from a different perspective, such as from the viewpoint of another witness or from a different location. - **How it works**: Changing the perspective can disrupt the influence of schemas (pre-existing knowledge or expectations), which might otherwise lead to distortions in memory. It also encourages the witness to think about the event in a more detailed way. - **Example**: A witness might be asked to describe the event from the perspective of the perpetrator or someone else present at the scene. **4. Change Order** - **Definition**: The witness is asked to recall the event in a different order, such as starting from the end and moving backward or recalling the event in a non-chronological order. - **How it works**: Changing the order of recall prevents the witness from using their expectations or knowledge of the event to fill in gaps or reorder the sequence of events. It also reduces the likelihood of memory distortions caused by assumptions. - **Example**: The witness may be asked to describe the event from the point of view of the aftermath (e.g., after the crime), and then work backward to the initial moment. **2. Enhanced Cognitive Interview (ECI)** The **Enhanced Cognitive Interview (ECI)** is a variation of the Cognitive Interview that was developed by **Fisher et al. (1987)**. It incorporates additional techniques to further improve the accuracy and detail of the witness's testimony. ***Key Features of the Enhanced Cognitive Interview** * 1. **Focus on the Witness's Emotional State**: The interviewer establishes rapport with the witness, ensuring they are comfortable and at ease. This reduces anxiety and encourages more accurate recall. 2. **Minimize Distractions**: The interview is conducted in a quiet and controlled environment, free from distractions, to enhance focus and concentration on the task at hand. 3. **Use of Open-Ended Questions**: Instead of leading or closed questions, open-ended questions are used to allow the witness to provide as much information as they can, without being prompted to answer in a specific way. 4. **Witness-Controlled Interviews**: The witness is given more control over the pace and direction of the interview. This encourages the witness to talk freely and in their own words, without being interrupted or forced to provide a specific answer. **3. Research Supporting the Cognitive Interview** The Cognitive Interview has been widely researched, and studies generally support its effectiveness in improving the accuracy of eyewitness testimony. ***Key Studies Supporting the Cognitive Interview** * - **Study - Geiselman et al. (1985)**: - In a laboratory experiment, participants watched a video of a violent crime and were then interviewed using either a standard police interview or a Cognitive Interview. The Cognitive Interview led to significantly more correct details being recalled (about 35% more accurate details) compared to the standard interview, showing its effectiveness in improving recall. - **Study - Fisher et al. (1987)**: - In this field study, experienced detectives were trained to use the Cognitive Interview. The results showed an increase in the amount of correct information gathered, with no significant increase in the number of errors, indicating that the Cognitive Interview was an effective tool for improving the quality of witness testimony in real-world settings. - **Study - Kohnken et al. (1999)**: - A meta-analysis of 53 studies found that the Cognitive Interview was more effective than standard interviews in improving the amount of accurate information recalled. However, it also found a small increase in the amount of incorrect information, highlighting the need for careful evaluation of the results when using the technique. **4. Evaluation of the Cognitive Interview** ***Strengths** * 1. **Evidence for Effectiveness**: Numerous studies (Geiselman et al., Fisher et al., Kohnken et al.) show that the Cognitive Interview leads to more accurate recall compared to standard police interviews. This supports its practical use in real-life settings, such as police investigations. 2. **Real-World Application**: The Cognitive Interview has been successfully implemented by police forces around the world, including in the UK and the US. It has been found to be a useful tool in improving the reliability of eyewitness testimony. 3. **Holistic Approach**: The Cognitive Interview's multi-technique approach, including context reinstatement, report everything, and changing perspectives, encourages more comprehensive and accurate memory retrieval. ***Limitations** * 1. **Time-Consuming**: The Cognitive Interview is time-intensive and can require more effort from both the witness and the interviewer. It may not always be practical in situations where there is limited time or when interviewing many witnesses in a short period. 2. **Training Costs**: Police officers must undergo extensive training to implement the Cognitive Interview effectively. This can be resource-heavy and may require a significant investment of time and money, especially if officers are not fully trained in its use. 3. **Potential for Increased Errors**: While the Cognitive Interview improves accuracy, studies like Kohnken et al. (1999) found a slight increase in the amount of **incorrect information** recalled. This suggests that while it enhances memory retrieval, it does not eliminate the risk of false memories entirely. 4. **Not Suitable for All Witnesses**: Some witnesses, particularly those who are anxious or have cognitive difficulties, may find the techniques of the Cognitive Interview overwhelming or confusing. This could limit its effectiveness in certain populations, such as young children or those with mental health issues. **Summary: Improving EWT through the Cognitive Interview** **Technique** **Description** **Example** ----------------------------------- ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- ------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- **Context Reinstatement** Encouraging witnesses to mentally revisit the context of the event. Ask the witness to recall the environment or their emotional state during the crime. **Report Everything** Encouraging witnesses to report every detail, even if it seems trivial. Ask the witness to recall sounds, smells, and small details that might seem unimportant. **Change Perspective** Asking the witness to recall the event from a different viewpoint. Ask the witness to describe the event from the perspective of the perpetrator. **Change Order** Asking the witness to recall the event in a different order (e.g., backward). Ask the witness to recall the event from the end and work backward. **Enhanced Cognitive Interview** A variation with additional techniques to improve accuracy (e.g., rapport-building, open-ended questions). Make the witness comfortable and use open-ended questions to avoid leading the witness. **[IN DEPTH MEMORY RESEARCH/ EXPERIMENTS]** **Multi-Store Model (MSM) of Memory - Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968)** The **Multi-Store Model (MSM)** suggests that memory is composed of three separate stores: **sensory memory**, **short-term memory (STM)**, and **long-term memory (LTM)**. Information flows sequentially through these stores, with attention and rehearsal playing key roles in transferring information from one store to the next. ***Supporting Studies for the Multi-Store Model** * **1. Peterson and Peterson (1959) - Duration of Short-Term Memory** - **Aim**: To examine the duration of STM when rehearsal is prevented. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants were shown trigrams (three consonants) and asked to recall them after intervals of 3, 6, 9, 12, 15, or 18 seconds. During the intervals, participants performed a distractor task (counting backward) to prevent rehearsal. - **Sample**: 24 psychology students. - **Results**: Recall accuracy sharply decreased as the delay increased. After 3 seconds, participants recalled around 80% of trigrams accurately, but after 18 seconds, recall dropped to about 10%. - **Conclusion**: STM has a limited duration of about 18-30 seconds without rehearsal, supporting the MSM\'s assertion that STM is a temporary store. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High internal validity** due to controlled conditions, ensuring that only the lack of rehearsal influenced recall. - **Evidence**: The distractor task (counting backward) effectively minimized rehearsal, isolating STM duration as a variable. - **Explain**: This provides strong empirical support for MSM's claim of STM as a brief, limited-capacity store. - **Link**: Findings support MSM's idea that rehearsal is essential for transferring information from STM to LTM. - **Critique**: **Low ecological validity** due to artificial task (trigram recall), which may not reflect natural memory usage. - **Application**: Practical implications for studying, suggesting that constant rehearsal is crucial to retain information in STM long enough to encode it into LTM. **2. Baddeley (1966) - Encoding in STM and LTM** - **Aim**: To investigate differences in encoding between STM and LTM. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants were presented with four word lists: acoustically similar, acoustically dissimilar, semantically similar, and semantically dissimilar. Participants recalled the lists immediately (testing STM encoding) and after a delay (testing LTM encoding). - **Results**: Immediate recall was worse for acoustically similar words, suggesting STM relies on acoustic encoding. After the delay, recall was worse for semantically similar words, indicating LTM relies on semantic encoding. - **Conclusion**: STM primarily encodes information acoustically, while LTM relies on semantic encoding, aligning with MSM's distinction between STM and LTM stores. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Experimental evidence for encoding differences** reinforces MSM's distinction between STM and LTM. - **Evidence**: Baddeley's controlled lab setting allowed isolation of acoustic and semantic encoding processes, strengthening reliability. - **Explain**: This supports MSM's assertion that STM and LTM process information differently. - **Link**: Findings align with MSM's framework, suggesting STM and LTM operate through distinct mechanisms. - **Critique**: **Limited real-world relevance** due to reliance on artificial word lists, which may not capture how people naturally encode complex information. - **Application**: Educators and learners can use this insight to improve recall by adapting encoding strategies (e.g., using semantic associations for deeper learning). **3. Bahrick et al. (1975) - Duration of Long-Term Memory** - **Aim**: To investigate the duration of very long-term memory (VLTM) using real-life information. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Nearly 400 participants aged 17--74 were asked to recall the names of their high school classmates by either free recall or through photo recognition and name recognition tests. - **Results**: Recognition tests were highly accurate even after 15 years (about 90%) and remained high (70--80%) up to 48 years after graduation, though free recall declined more significantly over time. - **Conclusion**: LTM can retain information for several decades, supporting MSM's idea that LTM has a potentially unlimited duration. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High ecological validity** as the study used real-life memories, making findings applicable to everyday memory retention. - **Evidence**: The use of meaningful, personal memories supports the view that LTM retains information more durably when it is relevant or frequently revisited. - **Explain**: This aligns with MSM's model of LTM as a durable, long-lasting store. - **Link**: The study supports MSM's claim that LTM can potentially last a lifetime, particularly when memories are frequently accessed or meaningful. - **Critique**: **Low control** over intervening variables (e.g., some participants may have kept in touch with classmates), which could influence recall accuracy. - **Application**: Findings highlight the importance of personal relevance in memory retention, suggesting that emotionally or personally significant events are more likely to be stored effectively in LTM. **4. Murdock (1962) - Serial Position Effect** - **Aim**: To examine the influence of list position on recall (primacy and recency effects). - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants were asked to recall lists of words presented one at a time. Each word was presented for a few seconds. - **Results**: Participants showed better recall for words at the beginning (primacy effect) and end (recency effect) of the list compared to words in the middle. - **Conclusion**: Primacy effect supports the transfer of early items to LTM due to rehearsal, while the recency effect suggests these last items are held in STM. This finding aligns with MSM's separate memory stores. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Robust experimental design** supports MSM's proposed structure. - **Evidence**: By controlling the presentation rate, Murdock demonstrated that the primacy effect relies on rehearsal while recency reflects STM retention. - **Explain**: This study underpins MSM's claim that different stores (STM and LTM) operate in parallel during memory tasks. - **Link**: Murdock's findings empirically support MSM's separate memory stores. - **Critique**: **Artificial list-learning task** may not reflect everyday memory processes, potentially limiting generalizability. - **Application**: Highlights the value of rehearsing early information, applicable in educational settings (e.g., revising early learned concepts first). **Critical Evaluation of the Multi-Store Model (MSM)** 1. **Strengths of MSM**: a. **Empirical Support**: Studies such as Peterson and Peterson and Baddeley provide strong experimental evidence for distinct stores in STM and LTM. These studies demonstrate that STM and LTM have different durations and encoding methods, supporting MSM's structure. b. **Practical Application**: MSM's focus on rehearsal and its impact on memory storage has practical implications in education and learning, as consistent rehearsal can help transfer information from STM to LTM. 2. **Limitations of MSM**: a. **Oversimplification of Memory**: MSM's linear and unitary structure for STM and LTM is critiqued as oversimplified. Research on the Working Memory Model (WMM) suggests that STM has multiple components, challenging MSM's view of STM as a single store. b. **Rehearsal Not Always Necessary for LTM Encoding**: MSM implies that rehearsal is essential for LTM encoding. However, **Craik and Tulving (1975)** found that deep semantic processing, rather than mere rehearsal, leads to more effective memory retention, which MSM does not account for. c. **Case Studies (e.g., Clive Wearing)**: Clive Wearing's case, where he has severe STM impairment but can still access some LTM functions (e.g., procedural memory), suggests that LTM is not a single unitary store as MSM proposes. 3. **Applications of MSM**: a. **Educational Strategies**: MSM's emphasis on rehearsal and the role of attention aligns with effective learning strategies, such as spaced repetition and active recall, which help transfer information into LTM. b. **Memory Rehabilitation**: MSM has influenced strategies in memory rehabilitation, especially for individuals with memory impairments, by focusing on improving rehearsal techniques to enhance STM **Working Memory Model (WMM) - Baddeley and Hitch (1974)** The **Working Memory Model (WMM)** suggests that STM is not a single unitary store (as proposed in the Multi-Store Model) but a system comprising multiple components that work together to process different types of information. ***Components of the WMM:** * 1. **Central Executive**: Directs attention and allocates resources to other components; has limited capacity. 2. **Phonological Loop**: Processes auditory information, with two subcomponents: a. **Phonological Store** (\"inner ear\"): Holds auditory information briefly. b. **Articulatory Process** (\"inner voice\"): Rehearses verbal information, enabling it to stay in working memory. 3. **Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad**: Processes visual and spatial information. 4. **Episodic Buffer** (added in 2000): Integrates information across the phonological loop, visuo-spatial sketchpad, and LTM, acting as a temporary store with a limited capacity. ***Supporting Studies for WMM** * **1. Baddeley et al. (1975) - Dual-Task Performance** - **Aim**: To demonstrate that separate components of working memory can handle different tasks simultaneously. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants performed two tasks at once: repeating a series of numbers (phonological loop) while completing a visual tracking task (visuo-spatial sketchpad). - **Results**: Participants successfully managed both tasks without interference, indicating that different stores were used. - **Conclusion**: Supports the WMM's claim of independent subsystems within STM. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: High **experimental control** makes findings reliable. - **Evidence**: The controlled conditions allowed for clear differentiation between tasks reliant on phonological and visuo-spatial components. - **Explain**: Supports WMM's claim that STM is composed of multiple independent systems. - **Link**: Contrasts with the MSM's simpler view of STM as a single store. - **Critique**: **Artificial dual-task** reduces ecological validity since real-world multitasking often involves more complex cognitive processing. - **Application**: Findings help in designing environments (e.g., classrooms, workplaces) that leverage different memory systems to reduce cognitive load. **2. KF Case Study (Shallice and Warrington, 1970)** - **Aim**: To investigate the memory impairment of KF, a patient with brain damage. - **Method**: Case study assessing KF's memory for different types of information. - **Results**: KF could process visual information relatively well, but his STM for verbal information was significantly impaired. - **Conclusion**: Supports WMM's division of STM into separate visual (visuo-spatial sketchpad) and auditory (phonological loop) stores. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High ecological validity** as it studies real-life memory processing in an individual with brain injury. - **Evidence**: KF's unique impairments offer insights into the specific functions of different STM components. - **Explain**: Real-life data from KF provide evidence that STM is not unitary, as WMM suggests. - **Link**: Supports WMM's distinct STM components, challenging the MSM's view. - **Critique**: **Limited generalizability** due to the single case study approach; findings may not apply universally. - **Application**: Influences tailored treatments for memory impairments, focusing on visual or verbal memory training depending on the affected component. ***Evaluation of WMM:** * - **Strengths**: - Supported by **neuroimaging evidence** (e.g., brain scans show different areas are active during visual and verbal tasks). - **Practical applications** in education, as teachers can design tasks that engage different components of working memory to prevent overload. - **Limitations**: - **Central Executive's vague definition**: Limited understanding of how the central executive functions or its exact capacity. - **Lacks explanation** of how each component interacts dynamically with LTM. **Types of Long-Term Memory (LTM)** **Tulving (1972)** proposed that LTM is not a single store but consists of separate types: **episodic memory**, **semantic memory**, and **procedural memory**. ***Types of LTM:** * 1. **Episodic Memory**: Personal experiences and events, stored with a sense of "time" (e.g., a recent vacation). 2. **Semantic Memory**: Knowledge and facts about the world (e.g., knowing Paris is the capital of France). 3. **Procedural Memory**: Skills and actions (e.g., riding a bike), often accessed without conscious thought. ***Supporting Studies for Types of LTM** * **1. Tulving et al. (1989) - Neuroimaging Evidence** - **Aim**: To investigate the separate brain areas activated during episodic and semantic memory retrieval. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants recalled episodic (personal) and semantic (general knowledge) memories while undergoing PET scans. - **Results**: Different brain regions were active for episodic and semantic memories (left prefrontal cortex for semantic, right for episodic). - **Conclusion**: Findings suggest that episodic and semantic memories are distinct types of memory. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Scientific credibility** due to neuroimaging techniques. - **Evidence**: PET scans provide objective evidence of distinct brain areas for each memory type. - **Explain**: This supports Tulving's theory that LTM has different types. - **Link**: Neuroimaging data reinforce the differentiation within LTM beyond the unitary concept. - **Critique**: **Limited sample size** due to the expense and invasiveness of PET scans, which may impact generalizability. - **Application**: Useful in designing treatments for memory loss, targeting episodic and semantic memory areas selectively. **2. Clive Wearing Case Study** - **Aim**: To investigate the effects of severe amnesia on different types of LTM. - **Method**: Case study of Clive Wearing, who suffered extensive brain damage affecting memory. - **Results**: Wearing retained procedural memory (could still play the piano) but had impaired episodic and semantic memory. - **Conclusion**: Supports the idea of separate LTM stores since procedural memory was unaffected while episodic and semantic memories were compromised. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Real-life evidence** makes findings highly relevant. - **Evidence**: Wearing's case demonstrates that LTM can be selectively affected, providing concrete evidence for LTM distinctions. - **Explain**: This supports the existence of separate systems within LTM. - **Link**: Confirms Tulving's theory that LTM comprises different stores. - **Critique**: **Single case limitation** restricts generalizability; the extent to which this applies to everyone is uncertain. - **Application**: Helpful in understanding amnesia, showing that procedural memory training may remain effective for patients with episodic memory loss. **Explanations of Forgetting** ***1. Interference Theory** * - Suggests that forgetting occurs because memories interfere with each other, particularly when they are similar. **Key Study: McGeoch and McDonald (1931) - Similarity and Interference** - **Aim**: To test the effect of similarity on retroactive interference. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants learned a list of words and then learned a second list that was either synonymous, antonymous, or unrelated. - **Results**: The more similar the two lists were, the greater the forgetting, with the highest interference for synonyms. - **Conclusion**: Supports the idea that interference, especially when memories are similar, leads to forgetting. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Experimental control** makes findings robust. - **Evidence**: Isolating similarity allowed clear examination of interference effects. - **Explain**: Supports the theory that similarity exacerbates interference, impairing recall. - **Link**: Reinforces interference theory as an explanation for forgetting. - **Critique**: **Artificial task** (memorizing word lists) may not represent complex memory processes. - **Application**: Useful in academic settings to avoid similar subject revision close together to reduce interference. ***2. Retrieval Failure due to Absence of Cues** * **Key Study: Godden and Baddeley (1975) - Context-Dependent Forgetting** - **Aim**: To test whether recall is better when the context at retrieval matches the context at encoding. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Divers learned lists of words either underwater or on land and were tested in the same or different environment. - **Results**: Recall was higher when learning and testing environments matched. - **Conclusion**: Supports retrieval failure theory, as context provides cues that aid memory recall. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High ecological validity** due to the naturalistic setting with divers. - **Evidence**: Real-life scenario of underwater context enhances validity. - **Explain**: Supports the role of context cues in retrieval. - **Link**: Reinforces retrieval failure as a key factor in forgetting. - **Critique**: **Limited applicability** as most learning doesn't involve such drastic environmental changes. - **Application**: Practical advice for studying, suggesting revising in a consistent environment may improve recall. **Eyewitness Testimony (EWT)** Eyewitness testimony is an important area in memory research, particularly in the context of legal proceedings. Research has shown that memory is not always reliable, as various factors can distort or affect recall. ***Factors Affecting EWT** * **1. Misleading Information** Misleading information, including **leading questions** and **post-event discussion**, can distort eyewitnesses\' memories of events. - **Key Study: Loftus and Palmer (1974) - Leading Questions** - **Aim**: To investigate whether wording of questions affects EWT. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants watched clips of car accidents and were then asked about the speed of the cars using different verbs ("hit," "smashed," etc.). - **Results**: Participants estimated higher speeds when the verb was more intense (e.g., "smashed" led to higher speed estimates than "hit"). - **Conclusion**: Leading questions can distort memory recall, suggesting that EWT may be unreliable. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: High **internal validity** due to controlled lab conditions. - **Evidence**: Wording was the only variable manipulated, ensuring clear causal links. - **Explain**: Supports the idea that memory can be influenced by external factors, like the way questions are asked. - **Link**: Provides strong evidence that EWT can be unreliable in legal settings if influenced by misleading information. - **Critique**: **Low ecological validity** due to artificial nature of watching video clips rather than witnessing a real accident. - **Application**: Crucial for law enforcement; careful phrasing of questions during interviews can help prevent false memories. - **Key Study: Gabbert et al. (2003) - Post-Event Discussion** - **Aim**: To explore the impact of post-event discussion on memory recall. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants watched a crime from different angles and discussed it with another participant who saw a different view. - **Results**: 71% of participants mistakenly recalled items from the other person's viewpoint, illustrating memory contamination. - **Conclusion**: Post-event discussion can significantly alter memory recall. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High ecological validity** as it mimics real-life scenarios where witnesses may discuss events. - **Critique**: **Potential for demand characteristics**, as participants may have guessed the study's aim. - **Application**: Legal guidelines can limit discussion between witnesses to prevent memory distortion. **2. Anxiety** Anxiety, particularly in high-stress situations, can either impair or enhance recall, depending on the circumstances. Two key theories explain this: - **Weapon Focus Effect (Johnson and Scott, 1976)** - **Aim**: To test if anxiety induced by a weapon affects EWT accuracy. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Participants witnessed a scene where a person held a pen (low-anxiety condition) or a bloody knife (high-anxiety condition). - **Results**: Participants in the high-anxiety condition (knife) had poorer recall of the person's appearance than those in the low-anxiety condition. - **Conclusion**: Anxiety and the presence of a weapon narrow attention, impairing memory of peripheral details. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: Supports **weapon focus effect** theory, showing how anxiety affects attention. - **Critique**: **Ethical issues** around creating anxiety; participants were exposed to distressing stimuli, though care was taken to mitigate harm. - **Application**: Important in legal cases involving weapons; highlights the limitations of relying solely on witness descriptions. - **Yuille and Cutshall (1986) - Real-Life Study on Anxiety** - **Aim**: To investigate the effect of real-life anxiety on EWT. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Interviewed witnesses of a real gun-shop robbery in Canada; anxiety levels varied based on proximity to the event. - **Results**: High-anxiety witnesses had more accurate recall, even months later. - **Conclusion**: Contradicts lab findings; in real high-stress situations, anxiety may enhance recall accuracy. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **High ecological validity** due to real-life setting, enhancing generalizability. - **Critique**: **Potential extraneous variables** such as witnesses' proximity to the event and emotional resilience. - **Application**: Informs court reliance on witnesses in high-anxiety situations. **Cognitive Interview (CI)** The **Cognitive Interview** (CI), developed by Fisher and Geiselman (1992), is a method to enhance eyewitness recall without leading questions. It is based on psychological principles of memory retrieval. ***Techniques of Cognitive Interview** * 1. **Report Everything**: Witnesses are encouraged to report all details, even if they seem irrelevant. 2. **Reinstate the Context**: Witnesses mentally recreate the event's environment (sights, sounds, etc.), aiding retrieval by context-dependent memory. 3. **Reverse Order**: Witnesses recount the event in different chronological orders to reduce schema-driven expectations. 4. **Change Perspective**: Witnesses describe the event from multiple viewpoints to prompt different memory cues. ***Supporting Studies for CI** * - **Fisher et al. (1989) - Field Test of CI** - **Aim**: To assess CI effectiveness with real witnesses. - **Method**: - **Procedure**: Trained detectives in CI techniques and compared their recall with standard interview techniques. - **Results**: CI elicited 47% more useful information than standard methods. - **Conclusion**: CI is more effective than traditional police interviews. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: High **practical relevance** due to real-world setting. - **Explain**: Results suggest that CI can significantly enhance recall accuracy without misleading or pressuring witnesses. - **Critique**: **Time-consuming** and resource-intensive, limiting its feasibility in all cases. - **Application**: Now widely used in police settings, CI helps in gathering more reliable eyewitness evidence. - **Milne and Bull (2002) - Effectiveness of Individual CI Techniques** - **Aim**: To determine which elements of CI are most effective. - **Method**: Tested each CI component separately and in combination. - **Results**: Using \"report everything\" and \"context reinstatement\" together produced better recall than other combinations. - **Conclusion**: Some CI techniques are more effective than others; combining certain techniques maximizes recall accuracy. - **Evaluation**: - **Point**: **Scientific approach** in isolating CI techniques provides practical insights. - **Critique**: **Variability in witness responses** to CI techniques may limit universal applicability. - **Application**: Police can adapt CI to prioritize effective elements, such as context reinstatement, based on each case. ***Evaluation of Memory Topics for A-Level A Responses*\*** 1. **Strengths Across Memory Topics**: a. Research on memory has strong **practical applications**, such as improving legal procedures and educational strategies. b. Studies often employ **controlled experimental methods**, enhancing reliability and establishing causal relationships. 2. **Limitations Across Memory Topics**: a. **Ecological validity** is often a concern in memory research; many studies (like Loftus and Palmer) are conducted in artificial settings. b. **Ethical considerations**: Studies on anxiety in EWT (e.g., Johnson and Scott) sometimes involve distressing scenarios, raising ethical issues about participant well-being. 3. **Applications in Real Life**: a. **Legal System**: Findings on EWT and CI have led to changes in police interview protocols and courtroom reliance on eyewitness testimony. b. **Education**: Insights from memory research guide effective study techniques, such as spaced retrieval, to aid learning and retention.