Guinther and Dougher (2013) PDF: From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy

Summary

This chapter provides an overview of clinical behavior analysis (CBA), tracing its historical development and exploring the translational research relevant to it. It discusses CBA therapies and the role of behavioral principles in understanding complex human behavior. The chapter also contrasts CBA with other approaches like cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT).

Full Transcript

Chapter 1 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy Paul M. Guinther and Michael J. Dougher In this chapter, we provide an overview of the his- Historical Overview of CBA torica...

Chapter 1 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy Paul M. Guinther and Michael J. Dougher In this chapter, we provide an overview of the his- Historical Overview of CBA torical and conceptual development of clinical CBA is a branch of applied behavior analysis (ABA) behavior analysis (CBA), characterize the transla- distinguished by the types of clients and clinical tional research most relevant to CBA, and describe problems addressed, the mode of therapeutic inter- the specific CBA therapies informing and informed vention, and the settings in which therapy is typi- by translational research. Although the term trans- cally conducted (Dougher & Hayes, 2000). We lational research is sometimes used in different should note that we are speaking here of the clinical ways (e.g., see Hake, 1982; Lerman, 2003; Mace, applications of ABA. ABA is also used extensively in 1994; McIlvane, 2009), for our purposes we a variety of other contexts including educational set- describe it as the investigation of the role of tings, organizations and industry, and athletic per- known behavioral principles in understanding formance enhancement. Within clinical contexts, more complex social and clinically relevant behav- ABA typically involves using direct contingency ior as well as the identification of behavioral prin- management to address the problems faced by, for ciples that are necessary to account for complex example, children and other individuals with brain human behavior. From this view, translational injury, autism, or other developmental disabilities. research is informed by both basic and applied Such ABA populations tend to be treated in environ- research, thereby bridging the gap between basic ments in which a great deal of control exists over findings and clinical applications at a faster pace reinforcement contingencies, such as residential than can be attained when relying on basic or treatment settings, schools, and hospitals. These applied research alone. features of ABA can be traced back to its emergence When it comes to CBA, the preponderance of in the early 1960s, when psychologists with devel- current translational research involves the experi- opmental and experimental backgrounds began mental analysis of verbal behavior and the behav- applying B. F. Skinner’s operant psychology to the ioral processes that underlie it. In particular, CBA problems faced by institutionalized patients and informs and is informed by translational research children in educational or therapeutic settings (see on rule governance, the indirect acquisition and Volume 1, Chapter 4, this handbook). The success of transformation of stimulus functions via derived direct contingency management procedures in such stimulus relations, and relational responding. settings helped behavior analysts carve out a niche Before delving more deeply into the relevant trans- that has carried through to the present day, espe- lational research and the therapies that constitute cially with respect to the ABA treatment of children CBA, we begin with a historical and conceptual with autism and other developmental disabilities. overview of CBA. DOI: 10.1037/13938-001 APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis: Vol. 2. Translating Principles Into Practice, G. J. Madden (Editor-in-Chief) 3 Copyright © 2013 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved. Guinther and Dougher However, for all of its successes with these popu- mental structures and processes (e.g., schemata, lations, ABA remains limited in its applicability to beliefs, expectancies, cognitive distortions) consid- the more traditional problems in clinical psychology ered responsible for maladaptive behaviors and and psychiatry, such as adult depression, anxiety ­psychological suffering. disorders, and adjustment disorders (Dougher & The adoption of this mentalistic–meditational Hayes, 2000; Kohlenberg, Tsai, & Dougher, 1993). orientation appeared to overcome the limitations of The clinical problems faced by verbally competent ABA (Kohlenberg et al., 1993). First, it made the adults often involve complex verbal processes that locus of behavioral causality portable. That is, the are not readily or adequately addressed by ABA’s problem of limited access to extratherapy contingen- direct contingency management procedures. In cies became mute; the target of intervention was addition, therapists in typical outpatient settings now the client’s mind, which was present in the have very limited access to or control over reinforce- therapy session. Clients could carry mental changes ment contingencies outside the therapeutic context. made within session out into their daily lives. In Although Skinnerian psychology readily lent itself to addition, the inherent mentalism of CBT accorded application in the form of ABA, the extension of well with the mentalistic view of behavior widely ABA to the treatment of traditional clinical problems held by members of Western cultures. Specifically, was conceptually and practically limited (Kohlen- many clients enter therapy with the belief that if berg et al., 1993). they can change their ostensibly problematic Given the limitations of ABA, behaviorally ori- thoughts and feelings, then they can lead happier ented clinicians in the 1950s and 1960s who were lives (Dougher & Hayes, 2000; Kohlenberg et al., interested in working with verbally competent 1993). Moreover, empirical studies of CBT were adults in outpatient settings turned to or were part reporting good outcomes, especially with depression of the growing behavior therapy movement. and anxiety disorders, which enhanced the appeal of Although behavior therapy shared with ABA an CBT’s mentalistic approach. As CBT gained in adherence to “empirically defined learning theory prominence, ABA was further relegated to circum- and conformity to well established experimental scribed settings and populations, and its application paradigms” (Franks & Wilson, 1974, p. 7), it in outpatient settings diminished. was based on the methodological behaviorism of While CBT gained prominence over ABA, CBA stimulus–response psychology (Mahoney, Kazdin, was slowly taking form and beginning to grow. & Lesswing, 1974). In the late 1970s, behavior ther- Unlike ABA, CBA is directly concerned with the apy went through a major change as stimulus– same kinds of clinical problems and treatment set- response theory merged with cognitive psychology. tings as CBT. CBA takes as its focus an accounting The change was not so much in terms of underlying of the behavioral processes governing publicly philosophy, because both were essentially mechanis- observable behavior and the covert behavior that tic and structuralistic (Dougher & Hayes, 2000), but makes up cognition. CBA is different from CBT in rather in terms of a new mechanical metaphor, the its epistemological foundation—radical behavior- computer, that guided theory and research. Mental- ism, which is philosophically incompatible with the istic and mediational accounts of behavior change methodological behaviorism of CBT. Thus, CBA (e.g., Bandura, 1969) began to emerge and quickly rejects the mentalistic and mediational causal grew into the cognitive–behavioral therapy (CBT) ­models of CBT. movement (Mahoney, 1974; Meichenbaum, 1977). One problem facing clinical behavior analysts Rather than grounding causality in operant and was that the experimental analysis of behavior respondent contingencies, CBT views thoughts, feel- tended to focus on relatively simple operants rather ings, and other mental or cognitive events as causal. than the kinds of complex behavior, especially ver- Specifically, mental events are said to mediate the bal behavior, common in clinical contexts. As an relation between the environment and behavior. example of the latter, clients frequently report Thus, the targets of intervention in CBT are the strong fear reactions to stimuli or events they have 4 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy never actually encountered. Likewise, individuals greater detail later in this chapter, we turn first to a diagnosed with depression tend to selectively attend conceptual overview of CBA and then to a discus- to and remember depressing events and often inter- sion of relevant translational research. pret seemingly neutral or even positive events nega- tively. Clients’ behavior often seems to be controlled Conceptual Underpinnings of CBA by rules, instructions, or inferences, even when they are in conflict with experience or with existing rein- As previously noted, CBA is based on the philoso- forcement contingencies. Laboratory studies of sim- phy of radical behaviorism. A more complete ple operant behavior provided little help for the description of CBA’s defining characteristics can be clinician attempting to understand these complex found in Dougher and Hayes (2000). Our focus here sources of control. is on the radical behavioral treatment of private Fortunately, in the 1980s and 1990s a resurgence events (i.e., perceiving, thinking, feeling, remember- of behavior-analytic research on verbal behavior and ing) and their role in a science and technology of cognition began to provide a nonmediational and behavior. A central problem presented by private functional account of the kind of complex behavior events is that, by definition, they are not publicly or that is commonly seen in clinical contexts. Thus, intersubjectively observable, which makes them dif- although the conceptual foundations of CBA can be ficult to study scientifically. The methodological found in Skinner (1957), Ferster (1972a, 1972b, behaviorist’s approach to this dilemma is to treat 1973), and Goldiamond (1974/2002), its empirical private and public events differently. Specifically, roots are equally planted in more recent areas of private events are considered mental or cognitive as research. This is not to say that CBA eschews direct opposed to overt observable behavior, which is con- contingency management procedures, but given its sidered to be physical. Mental events are defined focus on verbally competent clients in outpatient operationally and studied indirectly by inferring settings, its development was dependent on a more their existence from publicly observed responses to developed behavior-analytic understanding of verbal environmental manipulations. Methodological processes. behaviorism has been adopted by most of main- Perhaps the first fully articulated behavior- stream psychology (Hayes & Brownstein, 1986) and analytic outpatient therapy was Israel Goldiamond’s is particularly characteristic of cognitive psychology, constructional approach to therapeutic change which infers the properties of causal mental struc- (Goldiamond, 1974/2002; Schwartz & Goldiamond, tures (e.g., schemata) and their associated processes 1975). The more modern CBA therapies include (e.g., memory retrieval) from their products (e.g., functional analytic psychotherapy for enhancing the reported thoughts, feelings, and overt behaviors; see impact of therapeutic relationships (Kohlenberg & Hollon & Kriss, 1984, for a discussion of cognitive Tsai, 1991); integrative behavioral couples therapy structures, processes, and products). for increasing the quality of romantic relationships Insofar as it is based on the assumptions and (Christensen, Jacobson, & Babcock, 1995; Jacobson principles of cognitive psychology, CBT is directed & Christensen, 1996); dialectical behavior therapy toward changing those cognitive structures and pro- for the treatment of borderline personality disorder cesses thought to be responsible for feelings and and other severe problems (Linehan, 1993); behav- behavior. For example, much of CBT is based on the ioral activation therapy for the treatment of depres- ABC paradigm (Ellis, 1962, 1970), in which it is sion (Kanter, Busch, & Rusch, 2009; Lejuez, Hopko, conceived that antecedent environmental events (A) & Hopko, 2001; Martell, Addis, & Jacobson, 2001); lead to thoughts (B) that then cause subsequent and acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) for actions or emotions (C). Accordingly, replacing a the treatment of depression, anxiety, anger, and a maladaptive thought with an appropriate new variety of other quality-of-life concerns (Hayes, Stro- thought leads to a new and more preferable emotion sahl, & Wilson, 1999; see Chapter 18, this volume). or action. For example, a person at a party (A) may Although we describe each of these therapies in have the distorted thought, “No one here wants to 5 Guinther and Dougher talk to me” (B) and subsequently feel rejected (C). than working directly on the environment, verbal In therapy, that person may learn to replace his or behavior are stimuli that act on listeners (including her original distorted thought with a more appropri- the self as listener), whose reactions serve as con- ate alternative thought such as, “Or maybe these trolling consequences. From this view, thoughts are people are just shy” and subsequently feel curious verbal behavior that may influence other behavior rather than rejected. Presumably, repeated rational through their stimulus functions, although this corrections of maladaptive thinking eventually lead stimulus control is caused by environmental to changes in the core structures presumed to be contingencies. responsible for maladaptive thoughts (Beck, 1976; This radical behavioral view of private events and Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). At this point, verbal behavior has important clinical implications, the core structures cease to produce distorted cogni- both in terms of specific CBA interventions and in tive products, emotional stabilization is thereby pro- terms of therapeutic objectives and outcomes. As moted, and treatment is deemed successful. already mentioned, private events are not seen as Contrary to a pervasive misconception, radical causes and, therefore, are not the specific targets of behaviorists do not deny the existence of thoughts therapy. However, although CBA does not specifi- (Skinner, 1974). Instead, thoughts are seen simply cally target private events, they are not ignored. as instances of private behavior that, despite their Instead, the focus is on altering the conditions that lack of intersubjective observability, are not funda- give rise to these private events and their functions; mentally different from public behavior. As such, attempts are made to alter the function of thoughts behavior analysts consider thoughts to be dependent and feelings so that they are no longer discrimina- variables rather than independent variables or tive for maladaptive behavior. This fundamental dif- causes. Thus, an analysis of thoughts focuses on ference between CBA and CBT runs parallel to their their environmental determinants and their relation differing research bases. Grounded in the experi- to other behavior or, as Hayes and Brownstein mental analysis of behavior rather than in main- (1986) put it, behavior–behavior relations. This does stream methodological behaviorism, the various not mean that thoughts are considered unimportant CBA therapies are all informed by empirically or epiphenomenal. What it does mean is that there is derived behavioral principles and recent laboratory- no need in CBA to alter the occurrence or content of based research findings. We turn now to a discus- thought for a successful therapeutic outcome. sion of some of these findings. With respect to their influence on other behavior (i.e., behavior–behavior relations), a behavior- Translational Research Relevant analytic view of thoughts (memories, inferences, to CBA conclusions, etc.) is that they are instances of verbal behavior in which the speaker and listener are in the Although the entire body of research that consti- same skin (e.g., Skinner, 1957). As with private tutes the experimental analysis of behavior informs events, radical behaviorism and methodological CBA therapies, research on verbal behavior is behaviorism have fundamental and substantial dif- particularly relevant because it provides a behavior- ferences in their perspectives on verbal behavior. In analytic account of language and cognition. Rele- line with its inherent mechanism and structuralism, vant to this account is research on rule governance, methodological behaviorism focuses on language, or the indirect acquisition and transformation of stim- the structure or form of verbal behavior. In line with ulus functions via derived stimulus relations, and its functionalism, radical behaviorism defines verbal relational responding. behavior functionally (Skinner, 1957; but see Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, & Roche, 2001) and is not particu- Rule Governance larly concerned with its form. Defined functionally, A long-standing tradition within the experimental verbal behavior is behavior that is maintained by analysis of behavior is the use of nonhuman animal audience-mediated consequences. That is, rather studies to illuminate the basic principles of behavior 6 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy and conditioning. On the basis of the assumption of (e.g., Lippman & Meyer, 1967; Lowe, 1979). Inves- continuity of species, it was reasonably and widely tigations into the sources of these differences con- assumed throughout the 1950s that any principles verged on verbal processes, particularly those found to be applicable to nonhuman animals would involved in instructional control (see Baron & be equally as applicable to humans. However, start- Galizio, 1983, for a review), also known as rule- ing in the early 1960s, evidence slowly began to governed behavior (Skinner, 1966, 1974). accumulate showing that human participants were Instructions and rules are verbal stimuli that not behaving in ways that would be predicted by specify antecedent, behavioral, and consequent animal models. An investigation by Kaufman, Baron, components of operant contingencies (Skinner, and Kopp (1966) illustrated some of these unique 1966, 1974). They can be presented by others or be effects. In their study, three groups of participants self-generated. As examples, a participant in a were told that they would earn money according to human operant conditioning experiment may fol- a variable-interval, a variable-ratio, or a fixed- low the written instructions “Every two minutes, interval schedule of reinforcement. Participants in you can press the button in order to earn points,” or all three groups were then actually exposed to the a person may follow the self-spoken rule “After same variable-interval schedule of reinforcement. each meal, I should brush my teeth to avoid cavi- The investigators found that participants’ response ties.” That is, accurate rules tell people how to go rates were substantially influenced by what they had about behaving in ways that are likely to produce been told, which outweighed the influences of the desirable results (i.e., acquiring reinforcers or actual reinforcement contingencies. Such behavior avoiding punishers). However, following rules that in humans was perplexing. Unlike nonhuman ani- specify a relation among an antecedent, behavior, mals, humans in laboratory studies were shown to and consequence is sometimes incompatible with be more sensitive to instructions than to changes in effectively responding to operative contingencies programmed contingencies of reinforcement. (e.g., Catania et al., 1982; Galizio, 1979). For exam- Humans were also found to be quite susceptible ple, a person who sweeps his or her bedroom floor to instructions in other investigations. For example, on an hourly basis because of the rule that “cleanli- humans were found to be relatively insensitive to ness is next to godliness” (i.e., sweep on the hour so changes in programmed contingencies (e.g., Ader & as to maintain piety and avoid sin) may not connect Tatum, 1961; Barrett, Deitz, Gaydos, & Quinn, with the fact that it would take considerably longer 1987; Buskist, Bennett, & Miller, 1981; Catania, than an hour for the floor to become dirty or that Matthews, & Shimoff, 1982; Harzem, Lowe, & Bag- cleanliness is, in fact, not a path to godliness. Like- shaw, 1978; Matthews, Shimoff, Catania, & wise, if a person generates and follows the rule “If I Sagvolden, 1977; Shimoff, Catania, & Matthews, leave the house, something terrible will happen to 1981). Conversely, humans were often found to me,” staying in the house will be negatively rein- behave efficiently when provided with instructions forced by the absence of terrible events. The avoid- about the operative schedule contingencies; this effi- ance of these verbally described terrible events may ciency tended to develop more rapidly than when no also increase the probability of generating and fol- instructions were provided (e.g., Baron, Kaufman, & lowing other potentially maladaptive rules. Instruc- Stauber, 1969; Turner & Solomon, 1962; Weiner, tions and rules are clearly tied to both adaptive and 1962). Humans were furthermore found to exhibit maladaptive behaviors and are, therefore, of special patterns of schedule performance that were system- interest to clinical behavior analysts. atically different from those of nonhuman animals Initially, how behavior analysts were to account (e.g., Leander, Lippman, & Meyer, 1968; Lowe, Har- for findings that human laboratory behavior zem, & Hughes, 1978; Weiner, 1964, 1969), and appeared to be more susceptible to instructions and humans were found to exhibit greater intersubject self-generated rules than to programmed contingen- behavioral variability than nonhuman animals when cies was unclear. Such perplexing findings appeared exposed to the same programmed contingencies to lend credence to the mentalistic underpinnings of 7 Guinther and Dougher folk psychology and CBT (e.g., Ellis’s [1962, 1970] would be followed only as long as rule following ABC paradigm; Beck’s schema theory). In did not result in punishing consequences. particular, the findings appeared to support the view A second characteristic of an operant is that its that thoughts or cognition control adult human emission can be controlled by antecedent stimuli behavior to a greater extent than the environment. signaling the likelihood of response-contingent rein- Indeed, some theorists (e.g., Brewer, 1974) inter- forcement or punishment. In Galizio’s (1979) exper- preted such findings to mean that neither classical iment, human rule following came under antecedent nor operant conditioning had ever been conclusively stimulus control. When an orange light was on, the demonstrated in adult humans. Others (e.g., Ban- experimenter-provided rules were accurate, and fol- dura, 1971, 1974) took such findings to mean that lowing them resulted in the efficient avoidance of beliefs and expectancies were at least as important monetary losses. However, when a purple light was as reinforcement contingencies in controlling on, the instructions were inaccurate and following human behavior. them resulted in periodic losses. Participants were Clearly, if behavior analysis was to continue to not instructed about the meaning of these lights, but be a viable paradigm, it was going to have to deal they quickly followed the rules only when the with the fact that rules and instructions seemed to orange light was on, thereby demonstrating stimulus exert as much control over adult human behavior as control of rule following as an operant. programmed contingencies. This fact led to some Finally, Galizio (1979) hypothesized that rule fol- very interesting conceptual issues and empirical lowing was a generalized operant. That is, rule fol- research. In particular, behavior analysts had to lowing is an operant that generalizes to different develop a conceptually coherent account of verbal contexts that in some way resemble those in which stimuli, including self-generated and covert verbal rule following has been reinforced in the past. For stimuli (e.g., beliefs, expectancies, reasons, cogni- example, if rule following has been reinforced in the tions), and how these stimuli come to exert control presence of one’s parents while at home, this behav- over behavior. ACT and aspects of other CBA thera- ior is likely to generalize to rules given by the same pies emerged, interestingly, in the investigation of parents at the grocery store. When following other these issues, in large part through the research people’s rules (e.g., aunts and uncles, teachers, police efforts of clinicians who regularly observed the com- officers) is also reinforced, the operant is likely to plex verbal behavior of adult outpatients. generalize to still other adults. By the time human participants enter an experiment such as the one Rule following is operant behavior. An important arranged by Galizio, they have an extensive history experiment conducted by Galizio (1979) demon- of reinforcement for following rules that have been strated that rule following is an operant. Recall provided by authority figures such as experimenters. that an operant response is a class of behavior Given this history, it should not be surprising that affected by its consequences. In Galizio’s experiment, participants conform to experimenter-provided human participants tended to follow experimenter- instructions (see Milgram, 1963, for an extreme and provided rules as long as there were no negative troubling example of control by experimenter consequences for doing so. For example, in one instructions). condition, participants followed instructions to respond quickly to avoid monetary losses. This Classes of rule following. Not long after Galizio was not the most efficient way to respond (slower (1979) demonstrated that rule following was an responding would have avoided just as many operant, Zettle and Hayes (1982) suggested that losses), but it did work; nearly all losses were rule following could be usefully classified on the avoided. Rule following decreased in a differ- basis of the type of consequences that maintain it. ent condition, however, when responding at the One category of rule following is tracking (Hayes, instructed pace was insufficient to avoid all losses. Brownstein, Zettle, Rosenfarb, & Korn, 1986; Zettle Galizio argued that experimenter-provided rules & Hayes, 1982). Tracking is rule following that is 8 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy maintained by the consequences specified by the measured constriction, inhibition, conservatism, rule. For example, supposing one is instructed to intolerance of ambiguity, and perseverative tenden- meet one’s sales quota to earn a bonus, then fol- cies (Rehfisch, 1958a, 1958b); the questionnaire was lowing the rule (meeting the quota) is an example known to correlate with measures of social anxiety. of tracking if the reinforcer (the bonus) is respon- Next, participants were given a task with either sible for future rule following. The participants in accurate instructions (i.e., depending on the status Galizio’s experiment could be said to be tracking if of the signal light, press the button at different rates they continued to follow the instruction to respond to earn points) or minimal instructions (i.e., press quickly to avoid losses because doing so was fol- the button to earn points). At the beginning of the lowed by a loss-free period. task, points were delivered contingent on button A second class of rule following is called pliance presses, but then an extinction schedule was (Hayes Brownstein, Zettle, et al., 1986; Zettle & imposed in which responses were no longer fol- Hayes, 1982). Pliance occurs when one follows a rule lowed by points. Although accurate instructions because of one’s history of socially mediated conse- generally resulted in perseveration after the switch quences of being compliant or noncompliant. For to extinction, this effect was especially pronounced example, when a parent instructs a child to do his or among participants whose answers to the question- her homework, no natural consequence of the act is naire suggested high trait rigidity. Even in the mini- specified (e.g., boredom). Instead, if rule following mal instructions condition, trait-rigid participants occurs, it probably does so because of a history of were more likely to continue pressing the button socially mediated reinforcement for compliance (e.g., during extinction. Thus, highly rigid individuals the child may be allowed a snack or access to a appeared to be especially prone to pliance, even in favored television program) and a history of socially the face of discrepant feedback. mediated punishment for noncompliance (e.g., a In sum, insensitivity to programmed contingen- time out or loss of privileges). Participants in Gal- cies of reinforcement arranged in human operant izio’s study (1979) were likely predisposed to follow experiments appears to be a product of two types of instructions because of such histories. rule-following behavior. First, insensitivity may Given that rule following can be maintained by occur because of a history of reinforcement for social contingencies, one would expect that rule- tracking: Following rules has generally led to a host governed insensitivity to otherwise trackable contin- of naturally occurring reinforcers. Because of this gencies of reinforcement would vary across history, track-based rule following generalizes to the individuals as a function of the relative strength or human operant laboratory and precludes behavior potentiation of the social consequences for rule fol- that would be more efficient than the behavior spec- lowing, differential histories of reinforcement for ified by inaccurate experimenter-provided rules rule compliance, or both. In particular, individuals (e.g., Baron & Galizio, 1983; Galizio, 1979; Hayes, for whom pliance has been strongly reinforced (or Brownstein, Zettle, et al., 1986; Joyce & Chase, for whom noncompliance has been punished) may 1990). Second, contingency insensitivity may result be especially likely to follow rules even when rule from pliance or the differential sensitivity to the following leads to decreased rates of reinforcement social consequences for following rules and instruc- (Wulfert, Greenway, Farkas, Hayes, & Dougher, tions (e.g., Barrett et al., 1987; Hayes, Brownstein, 1994). Such histories have been posited to underlie Haas, & Greenway, 1986; Hayes, Brownstein, Zettle, the rigid cognitive expectations (i.e., rules) of indi- et al., 1986; for a review, see Hayes, Zettle, & Rosen- viduals who compulsively attempt perfectionism farb, 1989). Although investigated in the context of and to avoid mistakes (Beck & Freeman, 1990). human operant experiments, tracking and pliance To see whether individual differences in trait presumably account for the rule-following behavior rigidity would predict insensitivity to programmed exhibited by clients. contingencies, Wulfert et al. (1994) first assessed More important, a history of reinforcement for participants’ trait rigidity using a questionnaire that rule following would naturally lead individuals to 9 Guinther and Dougher generate and follow their own rules. To the extent provides much of the empirical foundation for that these self-rules lead to effective behavior, then the alternative therapeutic approaches used by rule generation and self-rule following would be CBA therapists. reinforced. In addition to such track-based rein- forcement for generating and following self-rules, Equivalence Relations and the Transfer there are inherent consequences for complying with of Stimulus Functions one’s own rules (self-pliance) just as there are for Stimulus equivalence is a behavioral phenomenon complying with rules provided by others (Hayes that emerges from certain natural conditions and et al., 1999). Doing what one says one is going to do laboratory arrangements (Sidman, 1994; Sidman & or what one thinks one should do has a long history Tailby, 1982). Roughly speaking, equivalent stimuli of social reinforcement in most cultures and is con- are those that, despite their physical dissimilarity, sidered a desirable and admirable personal charac- mean the same thing. That is, they are functionally teristic indicative of high intelligence (Wegner, interchangeable or do the same things. Otherwise 2002). Taken together, these two sets of reinforce- distinct stimuli can acquire common functions in ment contingencies can establish a sense-making many different ways, including through direct Pav- repertoire that is pervasive and leads individuals to lovian conditioning, through direct operant condi- generate rules and stick with them, even when they tioning, and through stimulus generalization (see are not particularly effective. Catania, 1998; Donahoe & Palmer, 2004). What is Persistent ineffective behavior is at least partly particularly interesting about equivalent stimuli is definitional of clinical disorders. Virtually all forms that they can acquire functions indirectly, in the of cognitive therapy hold that adhering to ineffective absence of direct conditioning or generalization. For rules underlies clinical disorders. CBT encourages example, for most English speakers, the word araña clients to adopt or generate new rules that, if prop- is not emotionally arousing. However, when told erly constructed, could place clients in contact with that araña is Spanish for spider, araña may acquire actual contingencies (e.g., through the correction of an eliciting function. That is, by establishing an cognitive distortions). How is CBA different? To equivalence relation between the words spider and answer this, we must first articulate how verbal araña, araña may indirectly acquire the functions of stimuli influence behavior. That is, although studies the word spider. More generally, words stand in an such as Galizio’s (1979) have demonstrated that equivalence relation with the events in the world rules can be discriminative for the operant behavior they reference or stand for, and by that we mean of rule following, such studies have not fully illumi- they take on many of the functions of their referents nated how rules alter the functions of the elements and vice versa. This is how rules can alter the func- of the contingencies they specify (see Dulany, 1968; tions of the elements of the contingencies they spec- Parrot, 1987; Schlinger, 1990, 1993; Zettle & Hayes, ify and how words can make people laugh, cry, 1982). An articulation of function alteration via ver- cringe, or fall in love. bal processes will go beyond the prevalent assump- Although stimulus equivalence is of interest in tion in mainstream psychology and the general its own right (a discussion of stimulus equivalence culture that thoughts are causal. Behavior-analytic and the methods used to study it can be found in research examining how verbal stimuli influence Volume 1, Chapter 16, this handbook), its clinical behavior is sometimes not intuitive. However, this relevance is most apparent in demonstrations of the research has widened researchers’ understanding of transfer of functions among stimuli that are mem- the causal conditions under which cognitions come bers of the same stimulus equivalence class (see to exert stimulus control, thereby revealing thera- Dougher, 1998). Laboratory studies of equivalence- peutic alternatives to the CBT approach to rules and based transfer of function typically start by using other cognitions. Research on the environmental matching-to-sample procedures to train and test determinants of function alteration via rules and some number of stimulus equivalence classes. cognitions, described in the following sections, A subset of one of the classes is then given a new 10 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy function, and investigators test to see if that func- emotion-eliciting functions in the absence of direct tion is indirectly acquired by other members of the training. They also suggest that explanations for this same class but not by members of the other classes. kind of phenomena go beyond the basic Pavlovian For example, Dougher, Augustson, Markham, or operant contingency. Greenway, and Wulfurt (1994) used a matching-to- In a second experiment, Dougher et al. (1994) sample training procedure to establish two four- demonstrated that the effects of extinction can also member classes consisting of unfamiliar figures (i.e., indirectly transfer among members of stimulus A1, B1, C1, D1 and A2, B2, C2, D2), in which the A equivalence classes. As in their first experiment, two stimuli were conditional for the selection of the B, four-member stimulus equivalence classes were C, and D stimuli (i.e., A–B, A–C, and A–D matching established and tested. Next, all of the Class 1 stim- was directly reinforced for the Class 1 and Class 2 uli except A1 (i.e., B1, C1, and D1) directly acquired stimuli). After participants reached the matching-to- positive conditioned stimulus (CS+) function by sample training criterion, they were given tests with- preceding mild electric shock. In Class 2, B2, C2, out feedback to determine whether the training had and D2 directly acquired negative conditioned stim- in fact established equivalence relations. Participants ulus (CS−) function by preceding shock-free peri- passed tests of symmetry (i.e., emergent B–A, C–A, ods. As expected, this procedure resulted in greater and D–A matching was demonstrated with the Class skin conductance responses to all of the Class 1 1 and Class 2 stimuli) and transitivity (i.e., emergent stimuli than to any of the Class 2 stimuli. The func- B–C, C–B, B–D, D–B, C–D, and D–C matching was tion of B1 was then directly altered by presenting B1 demonstrated with the Class 1 and Class 2 stimuli), in extinction (without shock) for six trials. To deter- thereby demonstrating the formation of two equiva- mine whether the new CS− function of B1 would lence classes (see Sidman, 1994; Sidman & Tailby, indirectly transfer to C1 and D1, all of the Class 1 1982). Next, a classical conditioning paradigm was and Class 2 stimuli (again, except the A stimuli) used to directly alter the function of the B1 and B2 were presented individually. Consistent with an stimuli. B1 acquired a positive conditioned stimulus indirect transfer of B1’s CS− function to same-class function because its presentation was followed with stimuli, no difference in skin conductance elicitation a mild electric shock. B2 acquired a negative condi- between the two stimulus classes was found after tioned stimulus function because it preceded a extinction of B1. In a final condition, B1 was recon- shock-free period. Skin conductance level served as ditioned as a CS+ by preceding shock, and all of the the measure of conditioning. After this direct acqui- stimuli were again presented to see whether this sition of stimulus functions by the B stimuli, skin reconditioning would transfer to C1 and D1. The conductance was measured when C1, D1, C2, and results confirmed that it did. D2 stimuli were presented in the absence of shock Continuing this line of research, Augustson and (A1 and A2 stimuli were not presented because they Dougher (1997) found that operant avoidance had been directly related to the B1 and B2 stimuli, behavior can be occasioned by members of a stimu- respectively, during the initial matching-to-sample lus equivalence class even when only one member of training). For most participants, C1 and D1 elicited the class has directly acquired response-evoking higher skin conductance levels than either C2 or stimulus function. In their experiment, two four- D2, despite the fact that C1 and D1 had never been member stimulus equivalence classes were trained paired with electric shock. That is, C1 and D1 indi- and tested (as described earlier), and B1 was estab- rectly acquired the fear-eliciting function of B1 lished as a CS+ using mild shock. Next, participants because of their common class membership, as pro- learned that they could avoid the shock that fol- duced by arbitrary matching-to-sample training. lowed B1 if they pressed the space bar on a com- In a similar study, Roche and Barnes (1997) demon- puter keyboard a set number of times within a set strated the transfer of sexual arousal through time period. No shock followed presentations of B2 stimulus classes. These studies begin to offer an (CS−), so no avoidance responses were required. explanation for how events can acquire and lose After avoidance was reliably occasioned by B1, 11 Guinther and Dougher participants were presented with the B, C, and D This is why a novel, poem, or slogan can move peo- stimuli from both classes in extinction (i.e., no ple to tears, make them laugh, or spur them to shock was delivered even if participants failed to action. This is why repeating the word calm can emit the avoidance response). Despite never having induce a state of relaxation and why thinking about been paired with shock and not being included in death can induce fear in some and relief in others, avoidance training, both C1 and D1 evoked avoid- even in the absence of any direct experience with ance responding in all participants. As was the case death. The equivalence relations between words and with B2, neither C2 nor D2 evoked avoidance their referents may also explain why clients fre- responding. quently cry when relating painful past experiences In addition to the transfer of emotion-eliciting or feel better after having talked about them (see and avoidance-evoking functions among members Pennebaker, 1997, for a review). Talking about of stimulus equivalence classes, investigators have emotionally painful events (CS+) in a neutral envi- demonstrated the transfer of a substantial number ronment (CS−) may promote extinction of the elic- of other stimulus functions, including contextual iting functions of those events. Equivalence relations stimulus control (Gatch & Osborne, 1989; Kohlen- may also explain how rules and instructions can berg, Hayes, & Hayes, 1991), conditional control alter the operant functions of relevant stimuli. For (Roche & Barnes, 1996; Wulfert & Hayes, 1988), example, the rule “Turn right at the corner to reach discriminative control (Barnes & Keenan, 1993; de your destination” alters the function of the actual Rose, McIlvane, Dube, Galpin, & Stoddard, 1988; corner such that it becomes discriminative for the Roche, Barnes-Holmes, Smeets, Barnes-Holmes, & behavior of turning right. In sum, these findings McGeady, 2000), conditioned reinforcement and begin to provide a behavior-analytic account of cog- punishment (Greenway, Dougher, & Wulfurt, 1996; nitively mediated stimulus functions and how indi- Hayes, Kohlenberg, & Hayes, 1991), ordinal control viduals come to respond both appropriately and (i.e., first, second; Green, Sigurdardottir, & Saun- inappropriately in novel contexts. ders, 1991; Lazar 1977; Lazar & Kotlarchyk, 1986; Wulfert & Hayes, 1988), and instructional control Other Stimulus Relations and the (McGuigan & Keenan, 2002). Taken together, these Transformation of Functions studies have provided convincing evidence that a Although equivalence relations are particularly rele- wide range of Pavlovian or operant functions can vant for discussions of how verbal stimuli affect transfer between equivalent stimuli via derived stim- human behavior, several stimulus relations other ulus relations. than equivalence can also alter stimulus functions To return to our earlier question of how verbal and influence behavior. These include opposition stimuli affect behavior, a plausible explanation and difference (Roche & Barnes, 1996, 1997; Steele develops if one assumes that words and other verbal & Hayes, 1991; Whelan & Barnes-Holmes, 2004), stimuli (whether spoken, written, drawn, thought, more than and less than (Berens & Hayes, 2007; pictured, imagined, etc.) are in equivalence relations Dymond & Barnes, 1995; O’Hora, Roche, Barnes- with corresponding events or objects (i.e., refer- Holmes, & Smeets, 2002; Reilly, Whelan, & Barnes- ents). The research just cited has suggested that ver- Holmes, 2005; Whelan, Barnes-Holmes, & Dymond, bal stimuli can elicit emotions and evoke operant 2006), before and after (O’Hora, Barnes-Holmes, behavior when the stimuli directly or indirectly Roche, & Smeets, 2004), and many others. Rela- acquire the stimulus functions of their emotion- tional frame theory (RFT; Hayes et al., 2001) was eliciting or operant-evoking referents. Words such developed on the assumption that a more compre- as death, vomit, murder, torture, love, peace, and calm hensive theory of verbal behavior would result from are all likely to have directly and indirectly acquired a more general account of stimulus relations and stimulus functions; they can elicit many of the same what is called arbitrarily applicable relational kinds of emotional reactions and evoke many of the responding (details follow). A complete account of same kinds of actions as their equivalent referents. RFT is beyond the scope of this chapter, but it is 12 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy important to provide an overview here to illustrate In addition, the child also learns that if one of two its translational and clinical relevance. stimuli is called bigger, then it can be derived that To respond to a relation or to respond relation- the other is called smaller. Similarly, if trained that a ally is to respond to one stimulus in terms of nickel represents a certain value (e.g., $0.05 worth) another. Most organisms can learn to respond to and told that a nickel is smaller than a dime, one can physical relations among stimuli, as when a rat derive that a dime has a larger value (i.e., more than learns to enter the brighter or darker of two runways $0.05 worth) despite its smaller physical size. or a pigeon learns to peck the brighter or darker of When an arbitrary stimulus relation has been two keys, regardless of their absolute brightness (see established with two or more stimuli, RFT proposes Reese, 1968; Skinner, 1953). Relational responding, that those stimuli participate in a relational frame. however, can also be established with arbitrary rela- Relational frames are defined by three characteristics tions among stimuli, for example, when humans (Hayes et al., 2001). The first is mutual entailment. learn that a name is equivalent to an object (e.g., the That is, given any relation, x, between stimuli A and word cup and a cup), that the numeral 10 is greater B, there is an entailed relation, y, between B and A. than the numeral 5, or that the word white is the Symmetry in equivalence relations is an example of opposite of the word black. These relations are arbi- mutual entailment. Given A = B, then the symmet- trary in that they are defined by social convention ric relation B = A is entailed. In relations other than rather than by the physical features of the stimuli. A equivalence, mutual entailment involves asymmetri- critically important feature of RFT is that arbitrary cal relationships. Given A < B, then the asymmetri- relational responding as typically seen in humans is cal relation B > A is entailed. itself hypothesized to be a contextually controlled The second defining characteristic of relational generalized operant class that emerges as a result of frames is combinatorial mutual entailment. Given a extensive formal and informal multiple-exemplar relation, r, between stimuli A and B, and another training over several years and in many contexts relation, x, between B and C, then there is an (Luciano, Becerra, & Valverde, 2007). That is, entailed relation, y, between A and C and an entailed responding relationally is differentially reinforced in relation, z, between C and A. Transitivity in equiva- certain contexts or in the presence of certain cues, lence relations exemplifies combinatorial mutual and over time, those contextual cues come to occa- entailment. Given A = B and B = C, then A = C sion relational responding. and C = A are entailed. Similarly, given A < B and As a naturalistic example of the learning history B < C, then A < C and C > A are entailed. thought to be responsible for arbitrary relational The final defining characteristic of relational responding, a child presented with a relatively small frames is transfer or transformation of functions. cat and a large dog is asked, “Which of these is big- The term transfer characterizes relational frames of ger?” and is reinforced for picking the physically equivalence, and the term transformation character- larger dog. The child is then asked, “Which of these izes relational frames other than equivalence. For is smaller?” and is reinforced for picking the physi- example, if A is a reinforcer and it is arbitrarily cally smaller cat. Later, the child may be presented established that A is the same as B, then the rein- with a relatively small toy car and a larger ball, forcing function of A will be indirectly acquired by asked again to identify which is bigger and which is B without alteration. That is, the function will trans- smaller, and reinforced for the appropriate selection. fer as reinforcement in accordance with the rela- After many such instances of reinforced responding tional frame of equivalence. However, the functions in the context of the words bigger and smaller, the that are indirectly acquired via relations other than child comes to occasion the correct selection equivalence vary depending on the specific type of between any two stimuli. The words bigger and relation that has been applied. For example, if A is a smaller can then be used to condition arbitrary rela- reinforcer and it is arbitrarily established that A is tionships, as when a child is reinforced for picking a the opposite of B, then the reinforcing function of A dime over a nickel when asked, “Which is bigger?” will be indirectly acquired by B with alteration. That 13 Guinther and Dougher is, the function will transform into punishment in accordance with the relational frame of opposition. To reiterate, by definition the transfer or trans- formation of stimulus functions results from engag- ing in the behavior of arbitrarily applicable relational responding. However, exactly which arbi- trary stimulus relations will influence behavior, and exactly which functions will transfer or be trans- formed, can be brought under contextual control (Dougher, Perkins, Greenway, Koons, & Chiasson, 2002; Wulfert & Hayes, 1988), permitting a high degree of behavioral flexibility with regards to rela- tional responding. Figure 1.1. Based on a popular training preparation (e.g., Dymond Roche and Barnes (1997) conducted an early lab- et al., 2007, 2008; Roche & Barnes, oratory investigation of function transformation in 1997; Roche et al., 2008; Steele & accordance with stimulus relations other than Hayes, 1991; Whelan & Barnes- equivalence. They began by replicating the Dougher Homes, 2004), the figure shows a relational network consisting of et al. (1994) finding that members of a stimulus directly trained (solid lines) and equivalence class could indirectly acquire eliciting derived (dashed lines) relation- functions, in this case showing a transfer of the ships, where S and O represent same and opposite relations, arousing functions of filmed sexual stimuli. Skin respectively. resistance, measured by polygraph, served as the dependent measure. With respect to nonequivalence relations and the transformation of stimulus func- classical conditioning procedure paired B1 and B2 tions, Roche and Barnes implemented a training with sexual and nonsexual film clips. When B1 pre- procedure resembling Steele and Hayes’s (1991) ceded a sexual film clip, its sexually arousing func- procedure for establishing a network of same and tion was indirectly acquired by C1, which was the opposite relations among various experimental stim- same as B1 through two same relations (i.e., B1 uli (see Figure 1.1). During pretraining, participants same A1 same C1). In addition, when B2 was paired learned to respond to the nonarbitrary relations with a sexual clip, its arousing function was indi- same and opposite in the presence of arbitrary same rectly acquired by C2, which was the same as B2 (S) and opposite (O) contextual stimuli, respectively through two opposite relations (B2 opposite A1 (the actual contextual stimuli were repeated charac- opposite C2). This combinatorially entailed trans- ters: !!!!!! and %%%%%%). For example, given S as formation of function occurred despite the fact that a contextual stimulus, a short line as a sample, and the tests for transformation (i.e., elicitation of a sex- short, medium, and long lines as comparisons, par- ual response) were conducted in the absence of the ticipants were reinforced for selecting the short line. S and O contextual stimuli. Thus, the eliciting func- In contrast, given this same problem but with O as a tions were transformed in accord with a network of contextual stimulus, participants were reinforced for derived and arbitrarily applied relations. selecting the long line. The S and O contextual stim- Using similar training procedures (see Figure 1.1), uli then were used to train a set of arbitrary relations Whelan and Barnes-Holmes (2004) established B2 between five other stimuli (i.e., A1 same B1, A1 same as a punisher, after which C2 functioned as a pun- C1, A1 opposite B2, and A1 opposite C2). After suc- isher and C1 functioned as a reinforcer. The rein- cessful direct training, several derivable relations forcing function of the C1 stimulus emerged even were tested and shown to be derived (i.e., B1 same though no member of the relational network had C1, B2 same C2, B1 opposite C2, and B2 opposite been directly established as a reinforcer. Thus, C1; see Figure 1.1). Next, a counterbalanced C1’s reinforcing function emerged through the 14 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy transformation of the B2 punishing function in In another study of the transformation of stimu- accord with the relation of opposite. lus functions via relations other than equivalence, More recent studies using comparable proce- Dougher, Hamilton, Fink, and Harrington (2007) dures (Dymond, Roche, Forsyth, Whelan, & Rho- demonstrated the transformation of discriminative den, 2007, 2008) have shown the transformation of and eliciting functions in accord with the relations avoidance and nonavoidance functions in line with less than and greater than. First, relational functions the relations same and opposite. As in earlier stud- were established for three visual forms labeled A, B, ies, a relational network was first established in and C by reinforcing across multiple training trials which A1 was the same as B1 and C1 but was the selections of the physically smallest, medium, or opposite of B2 and C2 (see Figure 1.1). In the sig- largest of three comparisons in the presence of sam- naled avoidance task that followed, participants ples A, B, and C stimuli, respectively. On each trial, learned to press a button in the presence of B1 to one of the samples would appear at the top of the prevent the presentation of aversive images and computer screen along with three visual compari- sounds; presentations of B2 were followed by the sons across the bottom that varied in size but were presentation of nonaversive images and did not lead otherwise identical. New comparison arrays were to avoidant responding. Thereafter, participants presented across trials, but regardless of the array, engaged in avoidance responding in the presence selections of the smallest comparison were rein- of C1 but not in the presence of C2, demonstrating forced when A was the sample, selections of the the transformation of avoidance and nonavoidance medium comparison were reinforced when B was functions. the sample, and selections of the largest comparison In a follow-up investigation, Roche, Kanter, were reinforced when C was the sample. This train- Brown, Dymond, and Fogarty (2008) first replicated ing was intended to establish the comparative rela- their demonstration of a network transformation tions A < B < C among the samples. of avoidance (B1 to C1, not C2) and nonavoidance After successful training, the B stimulus was pre- (B2 to C2, not C1) functions. They then disabled the sented for 30 seconds, and participants were avoidance response button and presented the exper- instructed to press the space bar on the computer imental stimuli in extinction. In a direct extinction keyboard at a steady rate. They were also told that group (n = 9), the response button was disabled the stimulus presented on the computer screen and presentations of B1 and B2 ceased to be fol- would sometimes change and to “press the bar at a lowed by images or sounds. In a derived extinction rate that you think is appropriate for each symbol group (n = 9), the response button was disabled and you see.” After a steady state of button pressing in presentations of C1 and C2 were not followed by the presence of the B stimulus was attained, partici- images or sounds. After the extinction phase, all pants were then presented with the A and C stimuli participants were exposed to all four stimuli, and while their response rates were recorded. Without avoidance responses were recorded in extinction. In further instruction and for all eight participants, the direct extinction group, only one participant did presentations of the A and C stimuli occasioned not avoid B1, whereas two participants did not avoid slower and faster responding, respectively, than the C1. Within the indirect extinction group, five partic- B stimulus. Thus, the discriminative functions of the ipants did not avoid B1, whereas eight participants A and C stimuli were transformed in line with their did not avoid C1. If these surprising results are rep- respective relations to the B stimulus. licable, they raise a question for further translational In the next phase of the experiment, the B stimu- research on extinguishing avoidance behavior: Are lus acquired a CS+ function by following it with a there circumstances in which derived exposure mild electric shock; skin conductance served as the (e.g., talking about a feared event) could produce dependent measure. After this direct conditioning, therapeutic gains that are comparable or even supe- the A and C stimuli were presented without rior to direct exposure (e.g., being in the presence of instructions, and skin conductance levels were a feared event)? recorded. Six of eight participants showed lower 15 Guinther and Dougher skin conductance response to the A stimulus and A detailed description of these processes is beyond higher skin conductance responses to the C stimu- the scope of this chapter (see Hayes, 1984, and lus than they did to the directly conditioned B stim- Barnes-Holmes, Stewart, Dymond, & Roche, 2000, ulus. All participants reported that they expected to for a more thorough accounting), but the important receive a larger shock to C than to B, despite the fact implication here is that most typically developing that C had never been paired with shock. One par- individuals are encouraged by their verbal commu- ticipant became so alarmed at the presentation of nity (culture) to act as if the self is a real entity. Fur- the C stimulus that she tried to remove the shock thermore, people are taught that the self roughly electrodes from her arm, fearing that the shock corresponds to their thoughts and feelings, which would be too severe. These results demonstrated are treated as causes of behavior. Taken together, that, along with their discriminative functions, the the sense of self as a real entity and the tendency to fear-eliciting functions of A and C stimuli had been explain behavior by appeal to private experience can transformed in line with their respective relation to be problematic and contribute to clinical problems. the B stimulus. Roche and Dymond (2008) reported As mentioned in our earlier discussion of rule similar results with sexually arousing stimuli. governance, rule following can be maladaptive, especially if one has a strong history of reinforce- ment for pliance and when rule following precludes Verbal Behavior and the Clinical contact with discrepant actual contingencies and Relevance of RFT consequences. Hayes et al. (1999, 2001) argued that According to Hayes et al. (2001), “The primary one particularly troublesome domain of maladaptive pragmatic purpose of the analysis of derived rules includes those that promote the avoidance of relations... is an analysis of the development of private experience or emotional avoidance. The verbal rules (e.g., through thinking, reasoning, and rules promoting this experiential avoidance can take problem-solving), and the use of verbal formulae to many forms but are largely rooted in cultural pre- guide behavior” (pp. 104–105). As mentioned, rules scriptions that certain thoughts and feelings are often allow for greater prediction and control of good and others are bad. Very few individuals would events, and the generation, derivation, and following argue with the classification of depression, sadness, of rules may therefore be reinforced as operant behav- anxiety, insecurity, fear, regret, and remorse as bad, ior (Hayes et al., 2001). At the same time, people’s and many sources of information, including parents, social environments often require and reinforce the friends, popular media, and mental health profes- formulation of complete and coherent narratives sionals, articulate and support that view. Most men- about their actions and the events in their lives. tal health professionals clearly see these emotions as Skinner (1953, 1974) suggested that this verbal undesirable, and most therapies are aimed at getting behavior is the basis of the development of self- rid of them. In contrast, from a CBA and especially awareness. Social demands for coherent narratives an ACT perspective, experiential avoidance is the about one’s own behavior require discrimination of source of many clinical problems. oneself from the rest of the environment and that The human tendency to constantly verbalize the narratives one generates fit with culturally (think) and problem solve as people interact with acceptable explanations. To respond to such ques- the environment around them extends to the world tions as “Where are you going?” or “How do you of private events, and people’s verbal behavior with feel?” or “What do you intend to do tomorrow?” the respect to these private events can itself become responder must first be able to discriminate him- or problematic. The trouble starts with a simple fact of herself from other objects and events in the environ- life: Negative thoughts and feelings are inevitable. ment. A fundamental assumption of both ACT and Living in a complex world without encountering RFT is that networks of derived stimulus relations frustration, failure, loss, sadness, fear, worry, insecu- play a critical role in this process and give rise to an rity, incompetence, and so forth is unimaginable. As abstracted, reified, and transcendent sense of self. people encounter such inevitabilities, however, 16 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy resulting statements such as “I feel depressed” do not CBA Therapies occur in isolation. Once said, “I,” which is assumed The existing CBA therapies, although differing in to be a real entity, is placed in an equivalence rela- domain of application and degree of emphasis on tion with “depressed,” which, by virtue of living in a the role of verbal processes in the development and culture in which depression is considered bad, is in treatment of clinical disorders, share core philo- an equivalence relation with “bad.” Through combi- sophical assumptions about the origins of human natorial entailment, “I” is then equivalent to “bad,” suffering, the means by which quality of life can be which, via function transformation, has the potential improved, and the goals or objectives of therapy. to elicit other negative private events and perhaps One commonality among the CBA therapies is a alter the functions of otherwise reinforcing events or focus on constructing effective repertoires rather activities (Dougher & Hackbert, 1994). than eliminating unwanted private events. The over- Many clients attempt to solve such problem in arching objective is to help clients lead rich and val- the ways they have successfully solved other prob- ued lives. A formal explication of this position can lems in their lives. In particular, one often rein- be found in Israel Goldiamond’s constructional forced problem-solving strategy is to remove or alter approach to therapy (Goldiamond, 1974/2002; the determinants of the problem. Coupled with a Schwartz & Goldiamond, 1975; see also Delprato, history of reinforcement for attributing causation to 1981; Fleming, 1984; Layng, 2009), which was per- the self and private events, “I am depressed” natu- haps the first outpatient therapy based primarily on rally leads to the conclusion that “I have to get rid behavior-analytic principles and assumptions. of this depression” and other related rules such as “I Israel Goldiamond (1919–1995) first developed his can’t do anything else until I don’t feel depressed” constructional approach with an eye toward protect- (Hayes et al., 1999, 2001). At an extreme, a rule ing the constitutional and human rights of individuals requiring the elimination of depressive feeling can living under institutional control (Goldiamond, entail an elimination of the self as a viable solution 1974/2002; Schwartz & Goldiamond, 1975). Within (i.e., suicide). Attempts to eliminate feelings of sad- institutions such as prisons and mental hospitals, the ness or depression can take many forms, but these prevailing ideology of the time was that the purpose of attempts are often not very helpful or healthy, pre- the institution was to establish total control over the cisely because the actions that are likely to alleviate lives and behaviors of those who were institutional- the depression, such as engaging in productive, val- ized, an approach that Goldiamond (1974/2002) ued activities, are contradicted by self-generated believed precluded the freedom to assent or dissent to rules or are depotentiated as reinforcers by the treatment. Therapeutic objectives in these institutions events that cause the depression in the first place tend to focus on eliminating socially undesirable (Dougher & Hackbert, 1994). In other words, behavior, where power differentials allow the institu- depressed individuals just do not feel like engaging tion, as opposed to patients, to determine which in previously reinforcing activities. Instead, they behaviors are desirable and undesirable. Goldiamond may attempt experiential avoidance (e.g., through argued that this total control approach to mental social withdrawal, excessive sleeping, self-medicating health treatment is not only ethically problematic but with alcohol or drugs) or spend time trying to think is also antithetical to the social purpose of ostensibly (ruminate) their way out of the problem. Paradoxi- therapeutic agencies. Alternatively, the needs of soci- cally, because of the equivalence relations between ety and the rights of clients are usually better met words and their referents and the attendant transfer through a sort of constitutional allegiance between of functions, attempting to verbally control private therapists and clients, in which those behaviors consti- events only makes them more likely to occur (see tuting therapeutic outcomes are agreed on by both also Wegner, 1994), and one may be better off therapist and client rather than dictated by the thera- accepting private experiences rather than struggling pist. This emphasis on respecting client autonomy is to alter their occurrence (Hayes et al., 1999; see especially relevant in outpatient settings, in which a Chapter 18, this volume). 17 Guinther and Dougher total control approach is neither pragmatic nor ethical. should be informed by the client’s values, not the Instead, a constructional therapist attempts to influ- therapist’s. By focusing on valued, personally mean- ence client behavior only insofar as it helps clients ingful activity as a therapeutic outcome, the con- bring their behavior under their own control and structional approach fulfills the ethical responsibility helps them achieve their own goals. of social institutions and professionals to provide More important, the objectives of a construc- therapeutic services while respecting and promoting tional treatment agreement are to be framed in clients’ autonomy (Goldiamond, 1974/2002). positive rather than negative terms (Goldiamond, Although Goldiamond (1974/2002; Schwartz & 1974/2002; Schwartz & Goldiamond, 1975; see Goldiamond, 1975) reported case examples of the also Delprato, 1981; Fleming, 1984). Using the successful use of a constructional approach, the U.S. Constitution as an analogy, Goldiamond approach represents a philosophical assertion of (1974/2002) noted that the framers sought to ensure what it means for an outcome to be therapeutic and domestic tranquility, not prevent unnecessary vio- is therefore not subject to empirical verification. lent revolts. More literally, CBA therapists seek to Rather, a constructional approach has been used ensure valued activity, not prevent undesirable successfully whenever treatment helps a client thoughts and behavior. Framing outcomes in nega- engage in behaviors that are consistent with the cli- tive terms is what Goldiamond referred to as an ent’s values, thereby improving the client’s quality eliminative approach. Under an eliminative of life by definition. approach, patients were often diagnosed according In addition to being rooted in a constructional to their socially undesirable patterns of behavior, approach and client values, CBA therapies are either with successful treatment defined in terms of the based on or are consistent with the philosophy of elimination of those behavioral patterns. The gen- radical behaviorism and its modern elaboration, eral problem with an eliminative approach is that functional contextualism (see Gifford & Hayes, 1999; there are typically many ways to eliminate an Hayes, 1993; Pepper, 1942; Skinner, 1953, 1974; unwanted behavior, but there is no guarantee that it Vilardaga, Hayes, Levin, & Muto, 2009). That is, will be replaced by desirable or more effective when CBA therapists conduct a behavioral analysis, behavior. For example, under an eliminative public and private behaviors are understood to be a approach, the successful treatment of stuttering function of their current and historical contexts, and would be defined as its elimination (or reduction). a behavioral analysis is said to be true or valid to the Sedation, punishment, and even surgical removal of extent that it leads to effective action with respect to the tongue and larynx would all eliminate stuttering, the achievement of specified goals. The inherent but none would be expected to result in more fluent monism and nonmentalism of radical behaviorism speech. Indeed, one far too frequently used therapy (see Dougher & Hayes, 2000) leads behavior analysts of the 1960s for the treatment of disturbing behav- to eschew explanations of behavior that rely on iors was frontal lobotomy, a surgical procedure that organismic states, traits, predispositions, thoughts, was highly successful at eliminating behaviors but beliefs, expectancies, or attitudes as sources of causa- far less effective at constructing effective repertoires. tion (see Hayes & Brownstein, 1986). The inherent In contrast, a constructional approach to stutter- pragmatism of radical behaviorism requires behav- ing entails promoting fluid speech. In the case of ioral theories and explanations to be cast in terms of depression, it amounts to switching from the goal of identifiable and manipulable environmental determi- eliminating depressive thoughts and feelings to the nants. As we tried to make clear earlier, however, this goal of promoting valued, personally meaningful does not mean that behavior analysts ignore private activity. The latter is important; a therapist could or verbal events or their roles as links in complex unilaterally impose a positively framed construc- chains of behavior. Instead, an adequate explanation tional program, but there would be no guarantee of behavior cannot stop with private events and must that this program would reflect the client’s values. explain both the occurrence of private events and Thus, the construction of therapeutic outcomes their influence on other behavior. 18 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy The third commonality among CBA therapies is Functional Analytic Psychotherapy that they tend to emphasize experience and experi- One of the first verbally based outpatient therapies ential learning over rules and rule governance. Cli- based on behavior-analytic principles and assump- ents are encouraged to set aside their verbal defenses tions is functional analytic psychotherapy (FAP). and justifications for their actions in favor of direct Developed by Robert Kohlenberg and Mavis Tsai evaluation of whether their actions are leading to (1991), FAP is based on the assumption that the valued outcomes. An important aspect of this pro- problems faced by verbally competent outpatient cess is clients’ discrimination of the actual results adults are mostly interpersonal, and the context of of their own rule following. As such, CBA therapists the therapy relationship is an ideal place to observe, do not seek to specifically develop better rules understand, and shape clients’ interpersonal behav- (thoughts, beliefs) for the client to live by. Instead, ior. Clients miss sessions, have insights, pick fights, therapy focuses on helping clients experientially express appreciation, fear disclosure, avoid certain evaluate their customary ways of behaving, includ- topics, lie, cry, relate, dissociate, and otherwise ing their ways of thinking, in terms of whether they engage in myriad behaviors that therapists can facilitate or detract from valued outcomes. directly observe, functionally analyze, and conse- The fourth commonality is that CBA therapies quate. Thus, the basic premises of FAP are that the encourage clients to accept experiences and situa- therapeutic relationship involves contingencies that tions that cannot be changed, that is, for which can be used to decrease the frequency of problem- there can be no effective action. Especially within atic behavior and increase the frequency of desirable ACT (Hayes et al., 1999; cf. Martell et al., 2001), behavior in vivo and that these changes may then this acceptance includes the painful or negative generalize to other interpersonal relationships thoughts, emotions, or memories that inevitably (Kohlenberg & Tsai, 1991). arise as humans interact in the world. It should not Although usable as a stand-alone treatment, FAP be interpreted as reflecting a cynicism or pessimism is more commonly used as a therapeutic adjunct to about the human condition or a kind of Nietzschean other primary treatments (Kohlenberg & Tsai, no-pain, no-gain philosophy. Rather, it is more in 1991). However, FAP assumes that an exclusive line with Zen Buddhist teachings; although pain is focus on the primary treatment may result in thera- inevitable, suffering is the result of verbal distortions pists missing opportunities to better understand the of the world and people’s beliefs that the world functional effects of their clients’ behavior as it should be different than it is. Human suffering is occurs in session and to shape client behavior exacerbated by struggling against what is and directly. In this sense, FAP resembles the direct con- attempting to control what cannot be controlled. tingency management procedures of ABA but is spe- Among the CBA therapies, ACT (see Chapter 18, cifically tailored to the context of outpatient therapy. this volume) has the most developed perspective on Much of the research to support the efficacy of acceptance. On the basis of basic laboratory research FAP consists of case studies (see Ferro García, on rule governance, transformation of function, and 2008). FAP-enhanced therapy has been conducted RFT, ACT makes a convincing case that negative with clients with anxiety disorders (Carrascoso private experiences acquire a disproportionate López, 2003; Kohlenberg & Tsai, 1998), personality amount of control over behavior and that because of disorders (Callaghan, Summers, & Weidman, 2003; the nature of verbal processes (relational frames), Holmes, Dykstra, Williams, Diwan, & River, 2003), the very act of trying to avoid these experiences nec- and chronic pain (Queiroz & Vandenberghe, 2006). essarily exacerbates them and detracts from valued Vandenberghe (2007) argued that FAP is also living. For this reason, learning to accept private well suited to facilitate exposure-based treatments events and see them for what they really are (i.e., for posttraumatic stress disorder and obsessive– verbal events) instead of what they appear to be compulsive disorder (see also Kohlenberg & Tsai, (i.e., the real condition of the world) is a key 1998; Kohlenberg & Vandenberghe, 2007). Further ­element of therapy. investigations have examined the use of FAP in 19 Guinther and Dougher treating depression after a breakup (Ferro García, difficulties, then, can arise when relationships lose Valero Aguayo, & Vives Montero, 2006), exhibition- their reinforcing functions or develop aversive func- ism (Paul, Marx, & Orsillo, 1999), and chronic pain tions. One way this occurs is through reinforcement and fibromyalgia (Vandenberghe & Ferro, 2005; erosion, which is essentially habituation to the Vandenberghe, Ferro, & Furtado da Cruz, 2003; see rewarding aspects of a relationship. Relationship dif- also Queiroz & Vandenberghe, 2006). FAP- ficulties can also arise from changes in the life cir- enhanced cognitive therapy (see Kohlenberg & Tsai, cumstances of one or both partners, such as having 1994) has been shown to be effective for the treat- children, loss or change of a job, increasing work ment of depression (Gaynor & Lawrence, 2002; stress, or financial difficulties. Incompatibilities that Kohlenberg, Kanter, Bolling, Parker, & Tsai, 2002), arise from such changes can lead a couple to vilify as has FAP-enhanced ACT (Dougher & Hackbert, each other’s differences and to withhold gratification 1994); FAP-enhanced behavioral activation also from each other in a polarization process (Jacobson shows promise in the treatment of relationship dis- & Christensen, 1996), further reducing the reinforc- tress (Manos et al., 2009). Other investigations are ing qualities of a relationship. A functional analysis beginning to reveal the active components of FAP of a given couple’s polarization process allows the (Busch et al., 2009; Kanter, Schildcrout, & Kohlen- clinician to sort topographically distinct problem berg, 2005; Kohlenberg et al., 2002), indicating that behaviors into functional response classes, which in vivo work leads to improved interpersonal func- helps to identify the underlying theme of the cou- tioning. Thus, across differing treatments and clini- ple’s primary conflict (Jacobson & Christensen, cal issues, FAP shows promise as a widely applicable 1996). After the problem theme is identified, the and effective interpersonal approach to therapy. partners are in a better position to collaborate on finding solutions that reduce polarization, promote Integrative Behavioral Couple Therapy each other’s needs, and improve the quality of their The use of behavioral principles in treating couples relationship (Berns, Jacobson, & Christensen, 2000). was originally developed in the form of behavioral As opposed to targeting particular behavioral marital therapy (Jacobson & Margolin, 1979), or topographies (as advocated by TBCT), IBCT change what is now called traditional behavioral couple strategies are directed toward placing clients in con- therapy (TBCT; see Christensen et al., 1995). With tact with contingencies that will reduce the fre- more than 20 randomized clinical trials of TBCT’s quency of the functional class of problematic efficacy, it is the most widely studied treatment for behaviors identified by the theme. For example (see marital distress (see Baucom, Shoham, Mueser, Dai- Berns et al., 2000), initiating sex may serve the func- uto, & Stickle, 1998; Christensen & Heavey, 1999; tion of promoting intimacy for one couple, but that Jacobson & Addis, 1993; Shadish & Baldwin, 2005). same topography could be a means of domination Despite this impressive level of empirical scrutiny, for another couple. For the first couple, initiations one of the pioneers of TBCT, Neil Jacobson, was not of sex could alter the frequency of behaviors from a fully satisfied with its reported clinical significance functional response class associated with attempts to (see Christensen et al., 1995). Impressed with achieve intimacy, whereas modifications of this increasing reports of success for acceptance-based topography would not benefit the latter couple in therapies, Jacobson and his colleague, Andrew their attempts to achieve greater intimacy. Given its Christensen, explored whether TBCT could be made more functional orientation, IBCT is more clearly more effective by including techniques for fostering aligned with modern CBA than is TBCT. acceptance. This gave rise to what is now called inte- Change strategies are common to both TBCT and grative behavioral couple therapy (IBCT; Christensen IBCT (Christensen et al., 1995; Jacobson & Chris- et al., 1995; Jacobson & Christensen, 1996, 1998). tensen, 1996; Jacobson & Margolin, 1979). First, Jacobson and Christensen (1996) reasoned that behavioral exchange procedures are used to encour- people engage in romantic relationships because of age couples to increase the number of reinforcing the reinforcement inherent in them. Relationship behaviors in their relationships, which can help 20 From Behavioral Research to Clinical Therapy counteract reinforcement erosion. For example, trial (RCT) of 21 couples experiencing clinically sig- each partner may make a list of the other partner’s nificant marital distress. Although statistical analy- wants, discuss their lists with each other and the ses were not reported, descriptive analyses showed therapist to plan behaviors around these wants, and that 64% of TBCT couples had improved or recov- then try some of the behaviors to see whether they ered by the end of therapy, whereas 80% of IBCT increase happiness or satisfaction in the other part- couples had improved or recovered. Christensen ner. Second, clients are given communication skills et al. (2004) conducted a large RCT with 134 mod- training, including speaking and listening skills, erately to severely distressed couples, concluding which can help counteract the polarization process. that TBCT and IBCT were equally as efficacious by The therapist may instruct and model extensively the end of treatment; 60% of TBCT and 69% of IBCT during the initial shaping of communication and couples were still reporting significant improve- reduce his or her level of involvement over time. ments at 2-year follow-up (Christensen, Atkins, Yi, Finally, problem-solving training may be used to Baucom, & George, 2006). Doss, Meng Thum, help couples define and communicate problems, Sevier, Atkins, and Christensen (2005) examined generate potential solutions in the form of alternate the 2004 RCT data and concluded that both thera- behaviors, evaluate and negotiate the alternatives, pies worked according to their differing theoretical and implement and evaluate planned change. mechanisms of change (see also Cordova, Jacobson, In addition to these change strategies, IBCT & Christensen, 1998). That is, TBCT decreased tar- includes strategies for promoting emotional accep- geted damaging behaviors and increased targeted tance of aspects of the other person that cannot be helpful behaviors to produce relatively quick thera- changed (Christensen et al., 1995; Jacobson & peutic gains, whereas IBCT gradually increased the Christensen, 1998), the idea being that closer rela- amount of acceptance for behaviors that could not tionships can be built on acceptance when change is be changed easily. Thus, although IBCT tends to not possible. Most couples will have problems that outperform TBCT, the available research would sug- cannot be changed, and IBCT therefore stresses that gest that IBCT and TBCT are equally efficacious but emotional acceptance strategies are as important, or qualitatively distinct therapies. even more important, than behavioral change strate- gies (Christensen et al., 2004). Moreover, acceptance Dialectical Behavioral Therapy may change the context of a relationship such that Marsha Linehan developed dialectical behavioral behavioral change is more likely, paradoxically therapy (DBT) during the 1980s while investigating being more effective than direct attempts at inducing the use of CBT for the treatment of parasuicidal cli- behavioral change (Jacobson & Christensen, 1996). ents (Linehan, 1993). More recently, however, DBT As mentioned, TBCT’s efficacy has been well has come to be most closely associated with the established (see Baucom et al., 1998; Christensen & treatment of borderline personality disorder. Duri

Use Quizgecko on...
Browser
Browser