W7 Content PDF

Document Details

SweetheartClematis3453

Uploaded by SweetheartClematis3453

Australian Catholic University

Tags

quantitative research study designs statistical analysis research methods

Summary

This document provides an overview of quantitative research study designs, including pseudo-randomized controlled trials, comparative studies, and case series. It also discusses important considerations for interpreting these studies, including potential biases and limitations.

Full Transcript

6.1 Quantitative -- but not an RCT   **Learning Objectives** By the end of this week you will be able to: - Determine NHRMRC Levels of Evidence for all quantitative study designs - Appraise the utility of small n research designs - Identify appropriate means of graphical representati...

6.1 Quantitative -- but not an RCT   **Learning Objectives** By the end of this week you will be able to: - Determine NHRMRC Levels of Evidence for all quantitative study designs - Appraise the utility of small n research designs - Identify appropriate means of graphical representation of data - Demonstrate basic Excel skills in calculating and representing data     **NHMRC Levels of Evidence: Levels III and IV** - Pseudo-randomized controlled trial (III-1) - Comparative study with concurrent controls (III-2) - Comparative study without concurrent controls (III-3) - Case Series with pre-/post-test outcomes   Are they worth your appraisal? The answer is... YES!       **Level III & IV Evidence : NHMRC** Level III: What are Pseudo-Randomised Controlled and Comparative Studies?   ![](media/image2.png)   **The Pseudo-Randomised Controlled Study Smith, Morrow & Ross (2015)**     Diagnosis: Test accuracy - Concurrent validity (when a standardised test is tested against another standardised test that we know works)   Alternate allocation (random ) Issues: Not blinded- for test accuracy , uneven grouping   N of 2 Communities: Matched pair and constrained randomisation (eg Public health)   **Allocation bias!** Multiple group case series Figure 8.3, Polgar and Thomas (2020)     - Trying to make sure community A and B are as similar as possible - Community A- didn't introduce program and community B was exposed to control program - Problems in allocation of group series   **What are Comparative Studies?** Time-Series Cohort - Calles quasi experimental designs\> especially interventions - Independent variable is the exposure of something to the group that either does or doesn't have a disease   Case Control - Time series cohort- starts at a period of time and moves forward, retrospective\> useful for groups that already have an impairment... e.g. we don't know who is and isn't going to get cancer - Look at the grouping- one group people who have the condition and other group people who don't have the condition - Look at other variables in society that could be linked to the condition - Case control- good for people who already have a problem or condition and is therefore useful for epidemiology and aetiology study   Historical control (III-3) - Reviewing a particular cohort and measure against controls that we already have information about at another time - Lower level of evidence than an RCT, however were looking at a group of people wth an intervention and comparing it to another intervention that is a gold standards - looking at types of outcomes... ensure new group of participants are very similar to the control group established in the other study     *Epidemiology* *Causal Relationships* - *Note: Both addressed in the graphic below* - *Vital in COMPARITIVE studies*       ![](media/image4.png)   RCT: people who are or not exposed to particular factors- outcome overtime... start now and move forward in time Case CS: looking back at a case controlled study with factors that were present of absent for people who have or don't have the disease   Cross section sudied- study of people observed at a single point in time   **What to check for...** **Cohort and Case Control studies** - Are comparison groups well described/defined? - Were measures of exposure and outcomes collected in the same way (preferably blinded) for each cohort? - Were all possible confounds identified? - What was the follow-up period? - If you are using historical controls, what differences are there in possible confounds?   **Pseudorandomised**\> - Appraisal: allocation bias is likely   **Comparative studies**: failure to clearly define the comparison groups. Are the comparison groups well defined or described in similarity   **Measures of exposure or outcome:** are they collected in the same way and or blinded in the cohort study   - Possible confounds: if there are no confounds in the study than something is wrong... - Follow up periods: was there enough time of a follow up period in cohorts. What happens to them as time progresses in perspective of the independent variable. - Historical controls: possible confound of historical controls and the experimental groups of current date cohort.       6.2 Are 'lower level' studies useful? **What are the Cons?** - Small n → cannot be generalised to larger populations - Sampling Bias\> small sample, bigger application to larger samples - Maturation Bias\> small study... likely to be a similar type of people - Investigator/clinician blinding not possible, no control - Measurement/Observation Bias \> problem: small amount of people and investigators know who they are - Client blinding not possible - Hawthorne effect\> occurs when people are in a study because they know if they are in research... expecting something good to come out of it - Note: rates of satisfaction   - Smaller number= smaller power - Do authors address there is bias or are they trying to minimise boas - Small studies- no control= backbone that RCTs are based on       - Different research questions ask for different designs - Risk factors with cohorts, surveys- accuracy of symptoms, frequency or prevalence- emotions etc. RCT- does this intervention work for greater populations   **Why not RCT?** - Some variables unchangeable (eg age, cancer, gender) - Ethical reasons (eg withholding Tx) \> experimental arm and no-treatment control (seen as withholding therapy from someone- may drop out) - Expensive \> tools to analyse statistics, very big to have power to prove effectiveness in treatment - Different research questions need different designs - RCTs measure change -- they rarely explain change \> dony explain why it occurs - RCTs measure populations -- dont address individual differences - RCTs -- biomedical model, not psychosocial   \"Absence of a randomised trial is not absence of evidence\" (Rosenbek, 2016) - Need to collect evidence to try and account for those changes we have to make in clinics   [PCTs are not cased on a psychosocial models because in allied health we are more concerned with behaviours rather than treatments and cures ]   **Smaller n Studies and Survey Designs** Case Series Surveys Single Case Experimental Designs   **Case Series (Level IV, NHMRC, 2008)**   **What?** - AKA clinical experiment (intervention or independent variable) - One participant group ('cases'), no controls - Prospective, with protocol \> protocol in collecting the outcome (blind raters- people who are blind and rate outcomes). What have they done to minimise biases?   **Why**? - 'Hypothesis-generating' (Hoffman et al., 2024) \> good idea that a modification to treatment to try and reduce the confounds as much as possible - **Pilot study**\> smaller studies guide bigger studies - Useful in targeting specific populations/quality improvement - Useful for complex populations 'Understanding unusual patient conditions' (Portney & Watkins, 2015) e.g. population with high comorbidities   **Surveys** What**?** - Questionnaires \> questioning to address quality improvement - Interviews - Can include open ended questions to collect qualitative data   Why? - Quantifies attitudes, beliefs - Characteristics of populations \> what are common wants, needs, barriers - Demographic characteristics   **Single Subject Experimental Designs (SSEDs) or Single Case Experimental Designs (SCEDs)** ![](media/image6.png)   - Experimental designs of study - Powerful for studies that only have one person: what makes them different? - Rigorous structure that involves an AB multiple design - Baseline period- no treatment and then go into a baseline period (with drawl design)   **Single Subject Experimental Designs (SSEDs) or Single Case Experimental Designs (SCEDs)** Multiple SSEDs = Meta-analysis Focus on specific dependent variable \> only looking at a very narrow thing and use small element to make change Protocol minimises other confounds 'Experimental' means not a case study \> unbiases scheme of cause and effect Empirical research SCED guidelines: The Single-Case Reporting Guideline In Behavioural Interventions (SCRIBE)   **What is a Case Study? Is it OK?** - AKA 'Phase I' study - Detailed observations - Behaviours and outcomes measured quantitatively - Natural, real-life \> done in clinics and community - ++variables = causal effects unknown - Good for complex clients/rare conditions - Good to observe **treatment processes**   6.3 Creating Useful Graphics: Building Your Excel Skills **Important Concepts and Skills for ALHT211** - How to create basic graphs, tables and charts - How to consider the right visual for the data   - Quantitative (**descriptive** & inferential) - Qualitative Data (themes)     ![](media/image8.png)     ![](media/image10.png)     **Presenting data** - **Bar graphs** can show nominal data (variables with qualitative characteristic e.g., ethnic group, gender), categorical data, or percentages as shown in figure 1. - The length of the bars are proportional to the frequency or percentage of the category. - You should be able to see precise values for the bars on the axis.     **Presenting data** - Histograms typically display the differences in frequencies or percentages among categories of interval/ratio variables. - The width of the bars are proportional to the width of the category and height proportional to the frequency or percentage of that category. - The histogram can be used to display the relative frequencies. - It can also show the nature of the **distribution** of the data; i.e., whether the data are **normally distributed.**   ![](media/image12.png)   **Presenting data** - Pie charts or pie graph stypically show the differences in frequencies or percentages among categories of nominal or ordinal variables. - The pie "slices" add up to 100% of the total frequencies     **Scatterplot** The scatterplot shows the distribution of two variables simultaneously. The relationship (or correlation) between two continuous variables is estimated.   ![](media/image14.png)   **Presenting data** - **Boxplots** display two common measures of the variability or spread in a data set. Range: largest value minus smallest value, and IQR (interquartile range) : Q3 minus Q1. - The bottom whisker goes from Q1 to the smallest non-outlier in the data set, and the top whisker goes from Q3 to the largest non-outlier. - The body of the boxplot consists of a \"box\" (hence, the name), which goes from the lower quartile (Q1) to the upper quartile (Q3). - If the data set includes one or more outliers, they are plotted separately as points on the chart. - Within the box the line is the median of the data set. Two vertical lines, called whiskers, extend from the top and bottom of the box.     ![](media/image16.png)

Use Quizgecko on...
Browser
Browser