When Do Stereotypes Come to Mind? (2003) PDF
Document Details

Uploaded by EndorsedAshcanSchool5449
University of Waterloo
2003
Ziva Kunda and Steven J. Spencer
Tags
Summary
This article presents a theoretical framework for understanding when people activate stereotypes and apply them to interact with people of stereotyped groups. It examines factors such as comprehension goals, self-enhancement goals, and motivation to avoid prejudice as influences on stereotype activation and application.
Full Transcript
Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 129, No. 4, 522–544...
Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 129, No. 4, 522–544 0033-2909/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.522 When Do Stereotypes Come to Mind and When Do They Color Judgment? A Goal-Based Theoretical Framework for Stereotype Activation and Application Ziva Kunda and Steven J. Spencer University of Waterloo The authors describe a theoretical framework for understanding when people interacting with a member of a stereotyped group activate that group’s stereotype and apply it to that person. It is proposed that both stereotype activation and stereotype application during interaction depend on the strength of compre- hension and self-enhancement goals that can be satisfied by stereotyping one’s interaction partner and on the strength of one’s motivation to avoid prejudice. The authors explain how these goals can promote and inhibit stereotype activation and application, and describe diverse chronic and situational factors that can influence the intensity of these goals during interaction and, thereby, influence stereotype activation and application. This approach permits integration of a broad range of findings on stereotype activation and application. As one interacts with a member of a stereotyped group—for use in judgments such as identification, categorization, and infer- example, an African American, a woman, a doctor—the stereotype ence about category members (Bruner, 1957). Empirically, how- of that group may or may not come to mind. When it does, it may ever, the distinction between the increased accessibility of catego- or may not color one’s impression of that person. These two ries and their increased use in judgment has sometimes been processes have been labeled stereotype activation, that is, the muddied because categories manifest their accessibility through extent to which a stereotype is accessible in one’s mind, and their impact on judgment. Therefore, increases in the accessibility stereotype application, that is, the extent to which one uses a of categories have typically been inferred from increases in their stereotype to judge a member of the stereotyped group. We ex- use (e.g., Srull & Wyer, 1979; for a review, see Higgins, 1989). amine what determines the activation and the application of ste- However, the extent of stereotype activation cannot always be reotypes and how these two processes relate to one another. We inferred from stereotype application. When perceivers apply a propose that both the activation and the application of stereotypes stereotype to a member of the stereotyped group, it can be assumed depend on three kinds of goals: comprehension goals, self- that they have also activated the stereotype, because a stereotype enhancement goals, and the motivation to avoid prejudice.1 We cannot be applied without first being activated. But when perceiv- explain how these goals can promote and inhibit the activation and ers do not apply a stereotype, it cannot be assumed that they have the application of stereotypes, and describe situational and chronic not activated it either, because people do not always apply their factors that can trigger these goals and enable their execution. activated stereotypes (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). For example, per- ceivers who encounter an Asian American woman may activate Conceptualizing and Assessing Stereotype Activation and the Asian American stereotype, thereby experiencing heightened Stereotype Application accessibility of stereotypic traits such as shy and intelligent, but they may refrain from applying this activated stereotype to this There has always been a clear theoretical distinction between individual, that is, they may not judge her as especially shy or the accessibility of knowledge structures such as stereotypes and intelligent. People may avoid applying an activated stereotype their use in judgment. Category accessibility was conceived as a because they are motivated to avoid prejudice (cf. Devine, 1989; state of perceptual readiness that makes the category available for Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). Nevertheless, its increased activation may still be detectable through implicit measures that tap other judgments that the perceivers cannot control or that they are not This research was supported by Grants 410-2002-0964 and 410-99-0993 motivated to control because they do not realize that their reactions from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and can reveal stereotype activation (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Grant 138483-98 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Several implicit measures of stereotype activation have been Council of Canada. We thank the stereotype discussion group at the developed. Increased accessibility of stereotypic attributes can be University of Waterloo for many helpful contributions to the development revealed by faster identification of stereotypic words (Kawakami, of these ideas, and Christian Jordan and Mark Zanna for comments on an earlier version of this article. 1 Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to either We view the terms motivation to avoid prejudice and goal of avoiding Ziva Kunda or Steven J. Spencer, Department of Psychology, Univer- prejudice as interchangeable. We use motivation throughout because that is sity of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1, Canada. E-mail: zkunda@ the term most commonly used in other articles on this topic (e.g., Dunton watarts.uwaterloo.ca or [email protected] & Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, 1998). 522 STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 523 Young, & Dovidio, 2002; Macrae, Bodenhausen, & Milne, 1995; can sometimes be aware that a stereotype is on their mind, and can Pendry & Macrae, 1996; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999), by an increase intentionally retrieve a stereotype when asked to (e.g., Devine, in stereotypic completions of word fragments (Gilbert & Hixon, 1989). The inhibition of stereotype activation may also result from 1991; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999; Spencer, Fein, Wolfe, Fong, & either automatic or controlled processes. People can intentionally Dunn, 1998), by faster pronunciation of stereotypic words (Mos- suppress unwanted stereotypic thoughts (Wegner, 1994), but may kowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, & Schaal, 1999), or by slower identi- also do so without intention or awareness (Moskowitz et al., 1999). fication of neutral words presented with to-be-ignored stereotypic Stereotype application, too, can occur either with or without in- words (Fein, Hoshino-Browne, Davies, & Spencer, 2003). The tention and awareness. People may use applicable stereotypes activation of stereotype-based affect can be revealed by faster intentionally to understand, explain, or predict the behaviors of evaluations of affect-congruent words and slower evaluations of others, but may sometimes form stereotypic impressions that they incongruent ones (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995). are not aware of (Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). Even when Stereotype activation can also be revealed by reactions to individ- people are aware of the contents of such impressions, they may be uals who do not belong to the stereotyped group; the activation of unaware of the stereotypic influences that produced them (Green- the stereotype of Black people, which includes the trait aggressive, wald & Banaji, 1995; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). can be revealed by an increase in the perceived aggressiveness of The distinction between stereotype activation and application a White person (Devine, 1989; Lepore & Brown, 1997) or by an should also not be confused with the distinction between associa- increase in aggressive behavior toward a White person (Bargh, tive thinking, which draws automatically on associations based on Chen, & Burrows, 1996; Chen & Bargh, 1997). These latter similarity, experience, and contiguity, and rule-based thinking, measures assess activation rather than application because their which is goal driven and draws on symbolic rules, causal analysis, targets were not members of the stereotyped group. If the same and logical inference (Sloman, 1996; E. R. Smith & DeCoster, measures were obtained for a target who did belong to the stereo- 2000). Stereotype activation and application can both result from typed group, they would assess stereotype application. either mode. Stereotypes can spring to mind automatically through Although research on stereotype activation has proliferated in well-learned associations to stereotypic cues (Bargh, 1999), but the last decade, most research and theory on stereotyping has they can also be retrieved through goal-driven inference that examined application, with the assumption that any stereotype highlights their potential to satisfy perceivers’ goals (Sinclair & application also implies stereotype activation (for reviews, see Kunda, 1999). Similarly, stereotypes can be applied through a Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Kunda & Thagard, 1996). Most research process of automatic, association-based assimilation (Devine, assessed explicit ratings of a member of the stereotyped group on 1989), but can also be applied as a result of rule-based motivated stereotypic attributes (e.g., Locksley, Hepburn, & Ortiz, 1982), reasoning (Fein & Spencer, 1997). though some assessed attitudes toward such a person from the positivity of behavior toward him or her (e.g., Gaertner & Dovidio, A Goal-Based Framework for Stereotype Activation and 1977). It should be noted that a measure assesses stereotype Application application only if it taps judgments about individual members of the group. Judgments about the group as a whole, such as re- Most theories of stereotyping have assumed that stereotypes will sponses to racism scales, can reveal the extent to which perceivers be on the minds of people dealing with members of stereotyped endorse the stereotype but not the extent to which they apply it; groups, and focused on understanding when these activated ste- one can believe that a group stereotype is generally true and still reotypes will be applied. However, although early research sug- refrain from applying it to certain members of the group (e.g., gested that anyone who encounters a member of a stereotyped Kunda & Oleson, 1995, 1997). group activates that group’s stereotype spontaneously (cf. Bargh, In sum, stereotype activation has typically been measured with 1999; Devine, 1989), it is now clear that such spontaneous stereo- implicit measures that assess the accessibility of stereotypic con- type activation is neither inevitable nor universal (cf. Blair, 2002); tent without also assessing reactions to a member of the stereo- it depends on perceivers’ prejudice, goals, cognitive resources, and typed group. In contrast, stereotype application has typically been learned associations (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Kawakami, Dovidio, measured with explicit measures that assess stereotype-based re- Moll, Hermsen, & Russin, 2000; Lepore & Brown, 1997; Sinclair actions to a member of the stereotyped group. Nevertheless, the & Kunda, 1999). distinction between stereotype activation and stereotype applica- More interesting, even if stereotypes are activated at the start of tion does not map neatly onto the distinction between implicit, an interaction with a stereotyped individual, this activation can automatic psychological processes and explicit, controlled ones; dissipate as the interaction continues (Kunda, Davies, Adams, & both activation and application can be either automatic or Spencer, 2002). In more than half a dozen studies, we have found controlled. no trace of stereotype activation in participants who had observed or interacted with a Black or an Asian individual for about 10 min Modes of Processing Underlying Stereotype Activation (Kunda, Davies, Hoshino-Browne &, Jordan, 2003). We also and Application found, however, that various events that occur at this later point in an interaction can bring the stereotypes back to mind. We propose Stereotypes can be activated automatically by stereotypic cues that events influence stereotype activation and application during (e.g., Bargh et al., 1996; Devine, 1989), but their activation can interaction through their impact on perceivers’ shifting goals. depend on the perceiver’s cognitive resources, a sign of controlled We suggest that both the activation and the application of processing (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). Moreover, although stereo- stereotypes at a given time depend on the extent to which perceiv- types can be activated without awareness or intention, perceivers ers are driven at that time by comprehension goals, self- 524 KUNDA AND SPENCER enhancement goals, and the motivation to avoid prejudice. Vari- Self-Enhancement Goals ants of these three goals have long been central to the motivational or functional approach to attitudes and social cognition, and have Self-enhancement goals encompass the needs to maintain, pro- been assumed to affect the retrieval and expression of attitudes and tect, and enhance self-esteem (Taylor & Brown, 1988; Tesser & beliefs (Herek, 1986; D. Katz, 1960: M. B. Smith, Bruner, & Campbell, 1983), affirm self-worth (Steele, 1988), and preserve White, 1956; Snyder, 1993). We propose that when stereotype valued aspects of self-concepts (Aronson, 1968); these relate to the application can help satisfy such goals, stereotypes are activated ego-defensive function of attitudes (D. Katz, 1960). Self- for that purpose. But when stereotype application can disrupt goal enhancement goals can motivate people to form particular impres- satisfaction, stereotype activation is inhibited to prevent such ap- sions of others; one may wish to think highly of someone who has plication. Goal-driven increases and decreases in stereotype acti- praised one and to disparage someone who has criticized one. vation often result in corresponding shifts in application. But People may activate and use applicable stereotypes that can sup- sometimes a stereotype activated to satisfy one goal is not applied, port such desired impressions (Fein & Spencer, 1997; Sinclair & if its application challenges other goals. Kunda, 1999; Spencer et al., 1998; for a review, see Kunda & People differ in their chronic strength of these goals (Kruglanski Sinclair, 1999), much like they activate and use other beliefs that & Webster, 1996; Plant & Devine, 1998; Taylor & Brown, 1988), can bolster their desired conclusions (Gollwitzer & Moskowitz, but goal intensity is also influenced by situational factors. For 1996; Kruglanski, 1996; Kunda, 1990). Social identity theory example, self-enhancement goals can be intensified by failure and (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) also predicted that one may derogate assuaged by a self-affirming experience (Fein & Spencer, 1997; members of out-groups to establish the superiority of the in-group Steele, 1988), comprehension goals can be intensified by surprise and so boost one’s self-worth. However, most research derived and weakened by time pressure (Kruglanski & Webster, 1996; from that theory did not address stereotype activation or applica- Wong & Weiner, 1981), and the motivation to avoid prejudice can tion because it examined reactions to minimally defined groups be intensified by a challenge to one’s impartiality and weakened with no preexisting stereotypes, or assessed overall judgments by an expression of egalitarian beliefs that establishes one’s rep- about real-life groups rather than about individuals who belonged utation as nonprejudiced (Fein et al., 2003; Monin & Miller, 2001). to them (for a review, see Brown, 2000). Fluctuations in goal intensity can prompt corresponding fluctua- When stereotypes can interfere with self-enhancement goals, tions in the activation and application of goal-relevant stereotypes. people may suppress them. People may inhibit stereotypes that might undercut their desired impression of a person; people mo- tivated to form a positive impression of someone may inhibit Comprehension Goals applicable negative stereotypes that contradict this impression Comprehension goals encompass the need to understand events, (Sinclair & Kunda, 1999) much like they inhibit other information reduce the complexity of the environment (Bruner, 1957), gain capable of interfering with goal completion (Gernsbacher, 1997; cognitive clarity (Jones & Thibaut, 1958), and form coherent Zacks & Hasher, 1994). impressions (Heider, 1958); these relate to the knowledge function of attitudes (D. Katz, 1960). Stereotypes serve these needs by The Motivation to Avoid Prejudice enabling perceivers to simplify and understand the huge amounts of social information that they confront and to make inferences The motivation to avoid prejudice may inhibit stereotype acti- that go beyond available information (Allport, 1954; Hamilton & vation and suppress the application of activated stereotypes. Atti- Sherman, 1994; Kunda, 1999; Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, tudes toward ethnic minorities and other stigmatized groups are 1994). Increases in the strength of comprehension goals can often fraught with conflict and ambivalence. Thus, White Ameri- prompt stereotype activation and application when stereotyping is cans may subscribe to symbolic racism, that is, the belief that assumed to aid comprehension (Kunda et al., 2003; Pendry & African Americans challenge core values of the Protestant Ethic Macrae, 1996). For example, a puzzling behavior may prompt the and, at the same time, also endorse egalitarian values that prohibit recruitment of applicable stereotypes that can help explain it prejudice (I. Katz & Hass, 1988; Kinder & Sears, 1981). Their (Kunda et al., 2002), much like any puzzling event can prompt the motivation to avoid prejudice may stem from an internally driven retrieval of information that makes it comprehensible (Kahneman desire to maintain an egalitarian identity or from an externally & Miller, 1986). driven desire to comply with egalitarian social norms (Dunton & Comprehension goals may also prompt stereotype suppression, Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, 1998). if stereotyping is assumed to disrupt comprehension. People may The internal motive relates to the value-expressive function of inhibit the activation and application of stereotypes if they con- attitudes (M. B. Smith et al., 1956) and encompasses the needs to sider them uninformative, as they often do when they have indi- express, verify, and act in accordance with central tenets of the self viduating information about a person (Leyens, Yzerbyt, & Schad- (Aronson, 1968; Swann & Read, 1981). The theory of aversive ron, 1992; Locksley et al., 1982; Yzerbyt, Schadron, Leyens, & racism (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986) focused mostly on internal Rocher, 1994). People may also inhibit the activation of one motivation to control prejudice: It assumed that people hold egal- applicable stereotype when another is more salient to permit a itarian values that prevent them from openly reacting in ways that coherent impression of the person (Macrae et al., 1995), much like could be construed as prejudiced, but that their negative feelings people inhibit other information that challenges comprehension and beliefs about certain groups may nevertheless color their such as distracters, inappropriate word meanings, and early but reactions to members of these groups when these reactions may false interpretations of sentences (Anderson & Spellman, 1995; also be construed in alternative, nonprejudiced ways. Put differ- Gernsbacher, 1997; Zacks & Hasher, 1994). ently, because of the motivation to maintain an egalitarian self- STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 525 image, activated stereotypes are applied only when their apparent supporting this explanation, the stereotype’s activation increases. application can also be understood to reflect other, stereotype- Thus, stereotypes that can facilitate goal attainment may be acti- unrelated factors. vated and used for this purpose. If one repeatedly uses stereotypes The external motivation to avoid prejudice relates to the social to satisfy a goal, this strategy may come to be triggered automat- adjustment function of attitudes (M. B. Smith et al., 1956) and ically whenever that goal is activated in the presence of stereo- encompasses the needs to present the self positively to others typed individuals (Spencer et al., 1998). (Jones & Pittman, 1982; Goffman, 1959), to fit in with others’ The contents of the stereotype can determine its goal relevance, expectations (Tetlock, Skitka, & Boettger, 1989), to avoid social because such relevance is established through causal reasoning disapproval (Crosby, Bromley, & Saxe, 1980), and to ensure about the implications of stereotypic attributes for goal attainment. smooth interpersonal interactions (Jones & Thibaut, 1958). Thus, For example, a negative group stereotype may corroborate a neg- people may suppress stereotyping because they are motivated to ative impression of a group member, but undermine a positive comply with perceived egalitarian norms that they do not person- impression. Therefore, it may be activated and used when self- ally endorse (Crandall, Eshleman, & O’Brien, 2002; Crosby et al., enhancement goals create the subgoal of disparaging a group 1980). member, but inhibited when the subgoal is to esteem a group People motivated to control prejudice may sometimes be unable member (Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). Similarly, when one is moti- to do so. Perceivers whose personal beliefs negate culturally prev- vated by comprehension goals to predict a person’s attributes, an alent stereotypes may try to avoid applying these stereotypes when applicable stereotype can contribute to this only if it contains they are activated, but the unwanted stereotypes may nevertheless information deemed relevant; people may activate and use the color their impressions when they lack the resources needed to stereotype of Asian Americans as interested in science to predict inhibit their application (Devine, 1989). an Asian student’s major (Kunda et al., 2003), but are unlikely to Most relevant research has focused on showing that the moti- use the stereotype of fat people for such predictions. vation to avoid prejudice can block the application of stereotypes Although the contents of a stereotype can determine whether it assumed to be activated (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986), but there is is activated or inhibited, a stereotype may also be inhibited simply some evidence that such motivation, like other goals, can also because it is a stereotype, regardless of its contents. Comprehen- inhibit the very level of stereotype activation (Fein et al., 2003; sion goals can prompt such inhibition when the presence of other Moskowitz et al., 1999). information about a person convinces perceivers that any stereotype-based inference will disrupt comprehension (Leyens et Processes Underlying Goal Activation and Satisfaction al., 1992). The motivation to avoid prejudice can also prompt such inhibition, when perceivers believe that any stereotyping may Situational factors can influence goal activation and satisfaction indicate prejudice (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). through both controlled, rule-based thinking and automatic, asso- ciative processes. People can take on goals intentionally, as they Overview do when asked to seek comprehension (Hamilton, Katz, & Leirer, 1980) or to avoid prejudice (Macrae, Bodenhausen, Milne, & The observation of a member of a stereotyped group can prompt Jetten, 1994; von Hippel, Silver, & Lynch, 2000). If people con- the activation of that group’s stereotype, if the perceiver pays sistently pursue the same goal in a given situation, they may come attention to the person’s group membership (cf. Fiske & Neuberg, to activate it automatically in that situation (Bargh & Chartrand, 1990). Stereotype activation can be increased if the perceiver is 1999; Mischel & Shoda, 1995). motivated to achieve comprehension or self-enhancement goals The first time a goal is triggered in a particular situation, one that can be satisfied by stereotyping the person. Such goals can may creatively devise a strategy for satisfying it. An activated goal also increase the application of the activated stereotype to the can activate a range of potential subgoals through which it can be person. However, an activated stereotype can also trigger the satisfied. Thus a self-enhancement goal may activate subgoals motivation to avoid prejudice, which can dampen stereotype acti- such as “affirm positive aspects of the self,” “seek praise,” or vation and can undercut and even reverse stereotype application. “establish superiority to others.” The subgoal that can most readily These influences are shown in Figure 1. be attained in the situation is adopted (cf. Fazio, 1990). In the The mere activation of a stereotype can influence judgments presence of a negatively stereotyped individual, one may choose to about a stereotyped individual through automatic assimilation, self-enhance by establishing superiority to this person because the much like activated stereotypes color impressions of individuals negative stereotype provides a handy means of doing so (Fein & who do not belong to the stereotyped group (Devine, 1989; Lepore Spencer, 1997). Similarly, a comprehension goal can activate a & Brown, 1997). Such automatic assimilation is depicted by the stock of potential explanations. The goal of comprehending a arrow leading directly from stereotype activation to stereotype disagreement with another may give rise to potential explanations application, and is not the focus of our discussion. Rather, we such as “my thinking was faulty,” “the other person’s thinking was focus on the other route, whereby a group stereotype is applied to faulty,” “we have different facts available to us,” or “we have an individual because of the individual’s membership in the group, inherently different perspectives.” The causal implications of and stereotyping is increased or prevented because of its implica- available information are weighed to determine which of these tions for perceivers’ goals. Our assumption that stereotypes can be explanations is most coherent (for details, see Thagard, 1989). applied through automatic assimilation and through rule-based When the disagreement is with a member of a stereotyped out- inference is shared by other models of stereotyping (Bodenhausen group, one may readily explain it as stemming from inherently & Macrae, 1998; Devine, 1989) and of social cognition (Chaiken different perspectives (Kunda et al., 2002). Through its role in & Trope, 1999). We also assume that the two kinds of processes 526 KUNDA AND SPENCER Figure 1. Overview of the model. A bold arrow with a plus sign leading from one component to another indicates that the first can activate the second, whereas a thin arrow with a minus sign indicates that the first component can inhibit the second. can operate simultaneously, and jointly influence one’s impression on context (e.g., Brewer, 1988) did not elaborate on this, most of an individual (cf. Fazio, 1990; Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999). likely because of the dearth of relevant research at the time. With The extent to which the perceiver pays attention to the person’s the recent explosion of research on stereotype activation, it is now category and is driven by each goal can be influenced by a host of clear that stereotype activation, like application, can vary from one chronic and situational factors. We assume that these factors exert situation to another. We therefore focus on both processes. their influence in parallel and jointly determine the perceiver’s We assume that the perceiver can entertain multiple goals si- final impression of the person. When they conflict, their impact multaneously, and that these can jointly determine stereotype depends on their relative strength. Thus, a strengthened self- activation and application. In contrast, most previous theories that enhancement goal can overcome ongoing motivation to avoid assumed that goals could influence stereotyping focused on only prejudice, resulting in the application of stereotypes that would one goal at a time. Theories that assumed that stereotypes often otherwise be curtailed (Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). These assump- dominate impressions paid little attention to the pressure to sup- tions underlie parallel-processing models of cognition (e.g., Holy- press stereotyping that can be exerted by the motivation to avoid oak & Spellman, 1993; Rumelhart & McClelland, 1986; E. R. prejudice (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). And theories Smith, 1996) and of social cognition (Kunda & Thagard, 1996; about the inhibitory role of the motivation to avoid prejudice paid Mischel & Shoda, 1995; Read & Marcus-Newhall, 1993; Read & little attention to the countervailing pressures to stereotype that can Miller, 1993). be exerted by comprehension and self-enhancement goals (Devine, 1989; Fazio et al., 1995; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986; Monteith, Comparison With Other Theories Sherman, & Devine, 1998). Our approach has a broader scope than earlier theories of Our approach is distinctive in its emphasis on situational influ- stereotyping and makes unique assumptions about underlying pro- ences on stereotyping. Others have focused predominantly on cesses. Whereas earlier theories focused predominantly on stereo- enduring influences such as perceivers’ chronic levels of motiva- type application (Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Kunda & tion to avoid prejudice (Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, Thagard, 1996), we also offer a systematic analysis of stereotype 1998) or to self-enhance (Tajfel & Turner, 1986), or the fit be- activation. Stereotype activation played at most a minor role in tween the features of a target and a stereotype (Fiske & Neuberg, earlier theories partly because of the prevailing assumption that 1990). We acknowledge such enduring influences but emphasize stereotypes would always be activated during an encounter with a the impact of temporary, situation-driven goals and provide a stereotyped individual (Bargh, 1999). This assumption was central detailed analysis of diverse situations that can intensify or weaken to Fiske and Neuberg’s (1990) model, which assumed that stereo- perceivers’ goals and, thereby, affect stereotype activation and types play a constant role in perceivers’ impressions and attributed application. variations in stereotype application entirely to variations in use of We differ from earlier theories in our assumptions about how individuating information (Fiske, Lin, & Neuberg, 1998). Even goals influence stereotyping. Key theories of stereotyping (Brewer, theories that recognized that stereotype accessibility could depend 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990) assumed that goals influence ste- STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 527 reotype application by determining the perceiver’s mode of that each factor exerts its impact by influencing the amount of processing—stereotype-based or attribute-based. The contents of attention that the perceiver devotes to the target person’s member- the stereotype are irrelevant to such determination. Broader models ship in the stereotyped group or by influencing the intensity of the of social cognition assumed similarly that goals influence stereo- perceiver’s self-enhancement goals, comprehension goals, or mo- typing through their impact on processing mode; they assumed that tivation to avoid prejudice. We typically infer that a goal has been stereotypes are used in heuristic but not in systematic processing, influenced by a given factor on the basis of other research relating regardless of their contents (Chen & Chaiken, 1999). In contrast, that factor to that goal or on the basis of a conceptual analysis of we assume that stereotype activation and application can depend the factor’s impact; direct measures of goal intensity have not been on the goal-relevance of the stereotype, which depends on its common in this research. The factors are organized according to contents; a goal can simultaneously prompt the activation of ste- the goals they are assumed to influence, and the direction of their reotypes that support it and the inhibition of those that thwart it impact on stereotype activation and application is shown. We (Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). Consistent with our view, the goal assume that factors that influence goal intensity exert comparable relevance of information has been shown to determine its retrieval impact on stereotype activation and application because stereo- and use for problem solving (Kolodner, 1993; Schank, Kass, & types are activated (or inhibited) to facilitate goal attainment by Riesbeck, 1994), analogical thinking (Holyoak & Thagard, 1997), promoting or preventing stereotype application. However, for motivated reasoning (Kunda, 1990), and categorization (Nosofsky, many factors, there is currently evidence of impact on only one of 1986, 1987; Stangor, Lynch, Duan, & Glass, 1992). these processes. We first review the factors shown to influence The contents of stereotypes do figure in Higgins’s (1996) view stereotype activation, and then turn to stereotype application. that the activation of a stereotype depends on its applicability, that is, on the overlap between the salient features of the stereotype and Stereotype Activation of the target person. We go beyond this view by focusing on the goal-directed causal inferences involving applicable stereotypes. Attention to the Target’s Category and Stereotype We note that an increase in the applicability of a stereotype can Activation sometimes prompt a decrease rather than an increase in its activa- tion, if the stereotype threatens current goals (Sinclair & Kunda, Perceivers who encounter a member of a stereotyped group will 1999). activate that group’s stereotype only if they are aware of the Our approach has most in common with that of Bodenhausen person’s category membership and have the cognitive resources and Macrae (1998) who also assumed that stereotyping is influ- needed to process this information. Any factor that diverts the enced by the three goals we focus on and also recognize both perceiver’s attention from the person’s category membership can excitatory and inhibitory influences. However, they assume that prevent the stereotype from getting activated in the first place, or each goal exerts its influence during a different stage of process- cause its initial activation to dissipate. ing, whereas we argue that each goal may affect both the activation Initial exposure. When one encounters a new person, the and the application of stereotypes. For example, they assume that person’s salient category membership can capture one’s immediate the motivation to comply with egalitarian norms influences only attention (cf. Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). This can the expression of stereotypes, whereas we show that it can also serve comprehension by permitting rapid classification. Classify- influence their activation (Fein et al., 2003). As well, we highlight ing a person as a member of a social group can entail the activation the role of the motivations to self-enhance, comprehend, and avoid of that group’s stereotype. There is considerable evidence that prejudice more explicitly, and pay greater attention to the circum- stereotypes can be activated spontaneously on exposure to a ste- stances that can influence the strength and impact of these goals. reotyped individual (for a review, see Bargh, 1999). In several We share their view that stereotypes can affect the interpretation of studies, a very brief video of an Asian or a Black person prompted behavior, but we do not focus in this article on these processes the activation of the relevant stereotype (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; because they have been reviewed elsewhere (Kunda & Thagard, Kunda et al., 2002; Macrae et al., 1995; Pendry & Macrae, 1996; 1996). Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). Such stereotype activation can be auto- matic; it can occur even when the exposure is subliminal or too Empirical Support brief to permit controlled processing. Thus, subliminal exposure to photographs of Black faces led non-African Americans to activate Because we aim to understand stereotyping in social interaction, the African American stereotype (Spencer et al., 1998), to behave we are especially interested in stereotype activation and applica- aggressively toward a White individual (Bargh et al., 1996; Chen tion within an encounter with a stereotyped person, where the & Bargh, 1997), and to misidentify tools as weapons (Payne, stereotype is activated (or inhibited) by the presence of that person 2001). Similarly, subliminal or very brief exposure to Black faces and is (or is not) applied to him or her. Much of our recent research led many White participants to activate negative affect (Dovidio, has examined stereotype activation and application within such Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997; Fazio et al., social situations, and the most direct support for our perspective 1995). Such effects were especially pronounced for highly proto- comes from this research. We also draw on research that examined typical Black faces (Livingston & Brewer, 2002). stereotype activation in less social settings, for example, in reac- Perceivers’ level of prejudice may determine whether they spon- tion to subliminal primes (Bargh et al., 1996; Spencer et al., 1998). taneously activate group stereotypes on encountering a group Table 1 presents the situational and chronic factors that have member. When primed with affectively neutral stereotypic asso- been shown to affect stereotype activation and application by ciates, participants high in prejudice activated stereotypes whereas perceivers exposed to members of a stereotyped group. We assume those low in prejudice did not (Kawakami, Dion, & Dovidio, 1998; 528 KUNDA AND SPENCER Table 1 Citations Pointing to Situational and Chronic Factors That Influence Stereotype Activation and Application by Influencing Comprehension Goals, Self-Enhancement Goals, and the Motivation to Avoid Prejudice Goals and the situational and chronic factors Studies in which the factor influenced Studies in which the factor influenced that affect them stereotype activation stereotype application Comprehension goals Impression task (⫹) Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000 Sensitive discussion topica (⫹) Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000 (⫺) Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000 Disagreementb (⫹) Kunda et al., 2002 Individuating information (⫺) Jordan & Kunda, 2000 (⫺) Locksley et al., 1980c Rasinski et al., 1985 Alternative stereotype (⫺) Macrae et al., 1995 Chronic need for structure (⫹) Neuberg & Newsom, 1993 Self-enhancement goals Receiving praise or criticism Stereotype supports desired impression (⫹) Sinclair & Kunda, 1999 (⫹) Sinclair & Kunda, 1999 Stereotype undercuts desired impression (⫺) Sinclair & Kunda, 1999 Threat to self-worth (⫹) Spencer et al., 1998 (⫹) Fein & Spencer, 1997 Greenberg et al., 1990 Self-affirming experience (⫺) Fein & Spencer, 1997 Muklincer & Shaver, 2001 Motivation to avoid prejudice Chronic egalitarian goals (⫺) Moskowitz et al., 1999 Challenge to impartiality (⫺) Fein et al., 2003 (⫺) Fein et al., 2003 Self-focus (⫺) Macrae et al., 1998 (⫺) Macrae et al., 1998 Salience of egalitarian norms (⫺) Fein et al., 2003 (⫺) Fein et al., 2003 Availability of excuses for prejudiced reaction (⫹) Darley & Gross, 1983 Yzerbyt et al., 1994 Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977 Snyder et al., 1979 Establishing egalitarian credentials (⫹) Spencer & Fein, 2000 Note. (⫹) ⫽ the cited study found the factor to increase stereotype activation or application; (⫺) ⫽ the cited study found the factor to decrease stereotype activation or application. a This factor also increases the motivation to avoid prejudice, which explains its negative impact on stereotype application. b This factor likely also increases self-enhancement goals, which may also contribute to its impact. c This finding was obtained in many other studies as well. For review, see Kunda and Thagard, 1996. Lepore & Brown, 1997; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997). In immediate exposure to stereotyped individuals on stereotype acti- contrast, prejudice did not affect stereotype activation in reaction vation is moderated by prejudice. to priming with negative stereotypic associates: Both high- and Passage of time. Initially activated stereotypes can dissipate low-prejudice participants activated stereotypes following such with time. Their activation may decay rapidly as one’s attention is primes (Devine, 1989; Lepore & Brown, 1997). It should be noted drawn to other incoming stimuli, as is the case for semantic that such research, which showed spontaneous stereotype activa- priming, whose effects can be eliminated by the mere interposition tion by stereotypic words such as Black, is moot as to whether the of several unrelated words between a prime and a target word same stereotypes will also be activated by encounters with actual (Neely, 1991). In one set of studies, participants viewed a video- members of the stereotyped group, who can always be categorized tape of a Black or White student discussing campus life (Kunda et in multiple alternative ways (cf. Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). Reactions al., 2002). Their activation of the Black stereotype was assessed to photographs of stereotyped individuals come closer to reactions after they had been watching the video for either 15 s or 12 min. to real people. A few studies examined the relation between Participants assessed within 15 s of their initial exposure to the prejudice and affective reactions to photographs of Black faces, Black target showed stereotype activation, but participants as- but these have yielded mixed results. White participants activated negative affect when primed with Black faces, but the extent to sessed after 12 min of exposure did not. The initially activated which they did so was uncorrelated with explicit measures of stereotypes had dissipated with time. Several additional studies prejudice in some studies (Dovidio et al., 1997, Experiments 1 and also found that stereotypes were not activated for perceivers who 3; Fazio et al., 1995, Experiment 1), was positively correlated with had been interacting with or observing a video of a Black or Asian prejudice in one study (Dovidio et al., 1997, Experiment 2), and individual for 10 –15 min (Kunda et al., 2002, 2003). Thus, even negatively correlated with prejudice in another (Fazio et al., 1995, though applicable stereotypes can be activated spontaneously as Experiment 2). This inconsistency may be due in part to the fact soon as one encounters a person, their activation may fade within that explicit measures of prejudice do not always tap actual prej- less than 15 min of further exposure to the person. As time unfolds, udice because of participants’ motivation to control their prejudice one’s attention shifts from the person’s category membership to (Fazio et al., 1995). Still, it remains unclear whether the impact of individuating information or to the demands of the tasks at hand. STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 529 Such shifts can reduce stereotype activation, as discussed in later prompt the activation of relevant stereotypes. Gender may come to sections. mind when one needs to predict a person’s ability to carry heavy It may be argued that despite the fading of stereotype activation, weights, and profession when one wishes to determine a person’s initially activated stereotypes will exert a lasting impact on ste- income. In one set of studies, participants took part in a structured reotype application, because a stereotypic impression formed on interview with an Asian or White confederate during which the activating the stereotype can persist even after the stereotype has confederate asked the participant stereotype-irrelevant questions faded. However, ratings of a videotaped Black person (Kunda et (Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000). Following 10 –15 min of such al., 2002) or behavior toward an Asian interaction partner (Dolder- interaction, half of the participants were given the goal of forming man, Kunda, & Spencer, 2002) following lengthy exposure an impression of their interaction partner’s personality and likely showed no evidence of such lingering stereotype application in career choice. Controls were given, instead, the goal of elaborating control conditions even though the stereotype was applied in other on the contents of their discussion. As may be expected from the conditions that prompted its reactivation. finding that stereotype activation can dissipate by the end of such Cognitive busyness. Any task that requires substantial cogni- a lengthy encounter (Kunda et al., 2002), controls interacting with tive resources may leave perceivers unable to pay attention to a an Asian confederate showed no activation of the Asian stereotype. person’s category membership, and thereby undercut stereotype In contrast, participants given the task of forming an impression of activation. In one set of studies, participants observed a brief their Asian partner did activate the Asian stereotype. Most likely, videotape of an Asian or White experimenter (Gilbert & Hixon, they recruited the stereotype so as to inform their impressions of 1991). Participants exposed to the Asian experimenter under nor- this person. mal circumstances activated the Asian stereotype, but participants This finding sheds new light on research on stereotype applica- made cognitively busy during the exposure did not. Other studies tion. One may be tempted to conclude that participants who showed that cognitive busyness can also prevent automatic stereo- applied a stereotype to a stereotyped individual had that stereotype type activation in reaction to subliminal priming with Black faces on their minds throughout their exposure to this individual. How- (Spencer et al., 1998). Thus, stereotype activation can require ever, this conclusion may be unwarranted because measures of sufficient attention and cognitive resources, and can be undercut stereotype application typically request participants to judge the when these are invested in other tasks. target on stereotypic dimensions. Such requests may suffice to The research demonstrating that cognitive busyness during an prompt the activation of stereotypes that might have otherwise exposure to a stereotyped person can undercut the stereotype remained inactive. Therefore, the widespread evidence of stereo- activation that would otherwise be triggered by this exposure has type application (for a review, see Fiske & Neuberg, 1990) may examined only very brief exposures to stereotyped individuals, and have led to a considerable exaggeration of the extent to which so suggests that perceivers who are too preoccupied when they stereotypes are routinely on perceivers’ minds. first encounter a stereotyped person may fail to activate the ste- Sensitive discussion topic. When issues sensitive to a stereo- reotype at that point. Cognitive busyness that arises later in the typed group arise during interaction with a group member, the interaction may also prompt the dissipation of initially activated stereotype becomes relevant to predicting that person’s likely stereotypes. Indeed, in studies that found no stereotype activation reactions, and may be activated for that purpose. A gentile inter- by the end of a lengthy exposure to a stereotyped person, partic- acting with a Jew may be reminded of that person’s Jewishness ipants were typically engaged during the exposure in other cogni- when Nazi war crimes come up, and a White American interacting tively demanding tasks such as assessing the suitability of the with an African American may be reminded of that person’s observed interview for an orientation program (Kunda et al., 2002) ethnicity when affirmative action comes up. or preparing for upcoming discussion topics (Hoshino-Browne & In one study, White Canadians took part in a 10 –15-min struc- Kunda, 2000). Such absorbing tasks may demand attention and tured interview with an Asian or a White confederate (Hoshino- distract one from focusing on a target’s group membership. If so, Browne & Kunda, 2000). On each round of the interview, the the demands of interacting with a stereotyped individual in social participant and the confederate were given 1 min to think about a or professional settings may often suffice to undercut the activa- newly introduced discussion topic. The confederate then inter- tion of initially activated stereotypes. viewed the participant about the topic. The first few topics were Although stereotype activation can decline during an encounter stereotype unrelated, but the final one was either a topic assumed with a stereotyped individual, various events can transpire at any to be sensitive to Asian Canadians—Canada’s immigration pol- point during the encounter to bring the stereotypes back to mind, icy— or a neutral topic—Canada’s health policy. After participants as discussed next. had thought about this topic, but before they had a chance to discuss it, their activation of the Asian stereotype was assessed. Comprehension Goals and Stereotype Activation Participants expecting to discuss the neutral topic with an Asian person showed no stereotype activation, but participants expecting When group stereotypes are considered relevant to understand- to discuss the sensitive topic with an Asian person did activate the ing and predicting the behavior of a group member, events that Asian stereotype. The thoughts listed by participants revealed that increase the salience of comprehension goals during an interaction they relied on their partner’s ethnicity to tailor their own reac- may prompt stereotype activation. In contrast, information that tions—participants expressed much less negativity toward immi- undercuts the perceived relevance of a stereotype to comprehen- gration when they expected to discuss it with an Asian than with sion may prompt its inhibition. a White person; most likely, they were trying to avoid any appear- Impression task. If during an interaction it becomes necessary ance of prejudice. It appears that perceivers who are no longer to determine one’s partner’s attributes or likely behavior, this may cognizant of the stereotype of their interaction partner’s group may 530 KUNDA AND SPENCER recruit that stereotype when a topic sensitive to that group comes individual may inhibit the activation of the stereotype, much like up so as to guide their expectations about their partner’s likely people inhibit other irrelevant information (Zacks & Hasher, reactions and sensitivities and so as to tailor their own behavior 1994). accordingly. In one set of studies, participants watched a video in which a It should be noted that the sensitive topic increased the extent to Black or a White student discussed stereotype-unrelated issues which stereotyping could serve perceivers’ comprehension goals for 12 min before mentioning that he had either failed a recent test and also increased their motivation to avoid prejudice, which (stereotype-consistent information) or excelled at it (stereotype- might have prompted the inhibition of stereotype activation (e.g., inconsistent information; Jordan & Kunda, 2000). Controls who Macrae, Bodenhausen, & Milne, 1998). The fact that the stereo- had viewed the Black student but received no information about type was activated anyway suggests that, for these participants, his test performance did not activate or inhibit the Black stereo- comprehension goals had a more powerful impact on stereotype type, but participants who had received diagnostic information activation than did the motivation to control prejudice. about the Black student inhibited the activation of the Black Disagreement. When one discovers a puzzling disagreement stereotype, and did so regardless of whether this information was with a member of a stereotyped group, one may recruit the ste- consistent or inconsistent with the Black stereotype. Thus, the reotype so as to solve the puzzle. As one struggles to understand acquisition of diagnostic stereotype-related information about a how it is possible for another person to hold a view that is so stereotyped person during an encounter with this person can high- different from one’s own, one may focus on the “otherness” of that light the irrelevance of the stereotype to the person and prompt its person as a likely explanation (Miller & Prentice, 1999). Different inhibition. social groups are often assumed to have different inherent essences Alternative stereotype. An applicable stereotype may also be (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Hamilton & Sherman, 1996; Medin, inhibited if it comes to seem irrelevant when a different applicable 1989; Rothbart & Taylor, 1992), and these may be used to explain stereotype becomes more salient. In one study, participants differences of opinion between members of different groups (Miller & watched a brief silent video of a Chinese woman behaving in a Prentice, 1999). For example, a man who disagrees with a woman manner that highlighted either her ethnicity (eating with chop- may assume that she sees things differently from the way he does sticks) or her femininity (applying makeup; Macrae et al., 1995). because she is a different kind of person, namely a woman. As a Participants activated the more salient stereotype but inhibited the result, he may activate the stereotype of women. competing one. Every person belongs to multiple stereotyped In one set of alleged jury-simulation studies, White participants groups; highlighting one of these group memberships can prompt first read about a court case and rendered a verdict (Kunda et al., the inhibition of stereotypes associated with others. 2002). They then observed a videotaped Black or White alleged In sum, stereotyping can sometimes facilitate comprehension fellow juror discuss irrelevant issues for 12 min before expressing goals and can sometimes interfere with such goals. When, during his own verdict, which was either the same or different from the interaction, perceivers experience an increase in the intensity of participant’s. Participants who observed the Black juror did not comprehension goals that can be achieved by using an applicable activate the Black stereotype if he had agreed with them but did if stereotype, they may activate it. And when they encounter infor- he had disagreed with them. Thus, the discovery of a disagreement mation that undercuts the relevance of an applicable stereotype to with a member of an out-group can prompt the activation of that comprehending their interaction partner, they may inhibit it. group’s stereotype. This may be because perceivers evoke the “otherness” of the stereotyped individual to explain his or her Self-Enhancement Goals and Stereotype Activation otherwise inexplicable reaction. Disagreement may also prompt stereotype activation because it Self-enhancement goals can sometimes be satisfied by dispar- challenges one’s worldview and, thereby, one’s self-worth. This aging or esteeming another person. Events that strengthen such may prompt self-enhancement goals that can also provoke stereo- goals may prompt the activation of stereotypes that support the type activation (Sinclair & Kunda, 1999). It is unclear whether desired impression and the inhibition of stereotypes that under- disagreement with the stereotyped individual prompted stereotype cut it. activation because participants were motivated to satisfy compre- Receiving praise or criticism from the target. When one is hension goals triggered by that individual’s puzzling behavior or praised, one may become motivated to think highly of the evalu- because they were motivated to satisfy self-enhancement goals ator so as to maximize the self-enhancing potential of the praise. triggered by the challenge to their worldview. Indeed, it may be But when one is criticized, one may become motivated to dispar- that both goals contributed to stereotype activation. age one’s evaluator so as to diffuse this personal threat. Perceivers Diagnostic individuating information. A stereotype may be may activate those applicable stereotypes that can support such a considered irrelevant to understanding a person when one has desired impression of their evaluator, and may inhibit those that other information about this person that seems more relevant. In can interfere with it. Motivated stereotype inhibition was found in such cases, one may inhibit the activation of the stereotype. People three studies in which participants observed a brief video of a view group stereotypes as irrelevant to their impressions of a group Black or a White person delivering positive or negative feedback member when they also possess individuating information about on their performance on a leadership test (Sinclair & Kunda, this person (Kahneman & Tversky, 1973). Indeed, they usually do 1999). Participants praised by a Black person inhibited the activa- not apply stereotypes to individuals about whom they also have tion of the Black stereotype, most likely because of its potential to diagnostic individuating information (for a review, see Kunda & undermine their desired positive impression of this person. For Thagard, 1996). Therefore, perceivers who obtain diagnostic indi- similar reasons, participants motivated to disparage their evaluator viduating information during an interaction with a stereotyped inhibited an applicable positive stereotype: Participants criticized STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 531 by a Black doctor inhibited the positive stereotype of doctors, women when primed with female faces, but participants with which, if activated, might have undermined their desired negative chronic egalitarian goals did not. A follow-up study revealed that impression of him. The notion that the stereotype inhibition ob- this lack of stereotype activation by chronic egalitarians was not tained in these studies was due to participants’ self-enhancement due to their lack of associations between female cues and the goals gained support from the finding that detached observers, stereotype of women but, rather, to their inhibition of that stereo- whose own self-views were not at stake, showed no such type. Thus, people with chronic egalitarian goals can inhibit ste- inhibition. reotype activation in the service of these goals when they encoun- Whereas people may inhibit those applicable stereotypes that ter stereotyped individuals. can interfere with their desired impression of an individual, they Challenge to impartiality. People may sometimes inadver- may activate those capable of supporting that impression. In ad- tently behave in a manner that can be construed as racist or sexist. dition to inhibiting the Black stereotype, recipients of praise from If bigotry runs contrary to their personal standards or to their a Black doctor also activated the stereotype of doctors, most likely understanding of societal norms, they may find such challenges to because this positive stereotype could be used to bolster their their impartiality disturbing and become motivated to avoid being desired positive impression of this person. Indeed, detached ob- or appearing prejudiced (Monteith, 1993; Sherman & Gorkin, servers showed no such activation. It should be noted that in all 1980). To satisfy this goal, they may try not to think about others these studies, participants criticized by a Black person activated in stereotypic terms, and inhibit applicable stereotypes. In one set the Black stereotype, but this stereotype activation could not be of studies, participants first received a challenge to their impar- attributed to self-enhancement motives because detached observ- tiality: they were told that a test they had taken previously sug- ers showed comparable activation, as people often do when ex- gested that they were racist (Fein et al., 2003). Controls received posed briefly to Black individuals (e.g., Bargh et al., 1996; Fazio racism-neutral feedback. Participants then read an article about a et al., 1995). The effects of self-enhancement goals on stereotype Black or White lottery winner, and performed a word-fragment- activation will only be observed in those circumstances where completion task in which words were preceded by subliminally stereotypes are not already activated for other reasons. presented Black or White faces (the race of the lottery winner and External threat to self-worth. Any personal threat may moti- of the primes was always the same). The word-fragment- vate people to disparage others so as to reaffirm their worthiness completion task assessed their activation of the Black stereotype. by establishing their superiority. They may activate applicable All participants were cognitively busy during the study. Like negative stereotypes for this purpose, and inhibit positive ones. In cognitively busy participants in other studies, recipients of neutral one study showing such motivated stereotype activation, partici- feedback primed with Black individuals did not activate or inhibit pants received positive or negative feedback on their performance the Black stereotype (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Spencer et al., 1998). on an intelligence test (Spencer et al., 1998). They were then made In contrast, participants who had been told that they were racist cognitively busy and performed a word-fragment-completion task inhibited the Black stereotype when primed with Black while being primed subliminally with Black or White faces. Par- individuals. ticipants who had received positive feedback did not activate the Similar results were obtained in studies where participants’ Black stereotype when primed with Black faces, replicating earlier impartiality was challenged by their own seemingly bigoted be- findings that cognitive busyness can undercut stereotype activation havior. In one study, participants learned that they had failed to (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991; Spencer et al., 1998). In contrast, partic- realize that a surgeon could be a woman (Sherman & Gorkin, ipants whose self-worth had just been threatened by negative 1980); in another, participants were forced to choose among sets of feedback did activate the Black stereotype when primed with responses that included only sexist options (Moskowitz et al., Black faces, despite being cognitively busy. Their self- 1999); and in another study, participants were led to believe that enhancement motivation was powerful enough to override the they had judged a man negatively because he was gay (Monteith, inhibiting effects of cognitive busyness on stereotype activation. 1993). These studies found that threats to impartiality that stem from one’s own behavior can make people feel uncomfortable and Motivation to Avoid Prejudice and Stereotype Activation increase their motivation to avoid prejudice (Monteith, 1993; Mos- kowitz et al., 1999; Sherman & Gorkin, 1980). Such threats may When stereotyping is assumed to indicate prejudice, people prompt people to purge their minds of stereotypic thoughts, but motivated to avoid prejudice may attempt to inhibit stereotypes there is no evidence yet for stereotype inhibition in such applicable to their interaction partners. circumstances. Chronic egalitarian goals. For some people, the goal of being Self-focus. When attention is focused on the self, personal and egalitarian is important and self-defining (Plant & Devine, 1998). societal norms become salient (Carver & Scheier, 1981). A height- Such people may attempt to purge their minds of stereotypic ened self-focus in the presence of a stereotyped person may thoughts when they encounter stereotyped individuals so as to therefore highlight antiprejudice norms and increase the motiva- maintain their egalitarian self-views. In one study, participants tion to adhere to them. This may prompt attempts to inhibit with and without a chronic goal of being egalitarian toward women applicable stereotypes (Macrae et al., 1998). One relevant study had to quickly pronounce a series of words, some of which were was based on the assumption that when one deliberately tries to stereotypic of women. (Moskowitz et al., 1999). The presentation suppress certain thoughts, these thoughts become hyperaccessible of each word was preceded by a very brief presentation (200 ms) later when one is no longer trying to suppress them (Wegner & of a male or a female face. The speed of pronouncing stereotypic Erber, 1992). Suppressed stereotypes should thus rebound once words served as a measure of stereotype activation. Participants perceivers stop trying to suppress them. Therefore, if perceivers who lacked chronic egalitarian goals activated the stereotype of who have an experience that can prompt stereotype inhibition later 532 KUNDA AND SPENCER show increased stereotyping, this rebound effect implies that their tivated stereotypes to a stereotyped individual (Gilbert & Hixon, stereotypes had been inhibited during the earlier experience. This 1991; participants busy only during application phase). On other logic was used to show that increased self-focus can prompt occasions, perceivers do not apply activated stereotypes (Sinclair stereotype inhibition (Macrae et al., 1998). Participants described & Kunda, 1999; detached observers). On yet other occasions, two different male hairdressers. Their self-focus during each de- perceivers engage in counterapplication of activated stereotypes scription was manipulated by exposing them to an image of (Fein & Spencer, 1998). We assume that perceivers will be more themselves, for heightened self-focus, or of a stranger, for a low likely to apply an activated group stereotype to a group member self-focus. Participants who provided the first description under when stereotyping this person can further their comprehension and high self-focus and the second under low self-focus showed a self-enhancement goals (cf. Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). There- rebound effect—their description of the second hairdresser was fore, events that trigger these goals may increase stereotype appli- more stereotypic than that of the first. Apparently, their heightened cation, whereas events that decrease preoccupation with these self-focus while describing the first hairdresser prompted them to goals or undercut the extent to which stereotyping can satisfy them inhibit unwanted stereotypic thoughts; the suppressed stereotype may reduce stereotype application. However, perceivers are likely then became hyperaccessible once the self was no longer in focus, to avoid applying activated stereotypes when they are driven by and colored their judgments of the second hairdresser. Consistent the motivation to avoid prejudice (cf. Devine, 1989). Therefore, with this interpretation, other participants who described both factors that increase this motivation may reduce stereotype appli- targets under either high or low self-focus did not show a similar cation, whereas factors that decrease it may increase stereotype rebound effect when judging the second target. Heightened self- application. However, controlling prejudice is an effortful process focus may prompt stereotype inhibition, most likely by increasing that can be disrupted when the perceiver lacks sufficient resources the motivation to adhere to antiprejudice norms. (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991). Therefore, the impact of the motivation Salience of egalitarian norms. The behavior of others can to avoid prejudice on stereotype application may be moderated by highlight social norms; when others frown upon a bigoted joke the availability of cognitive resources. rather than laugh at it, it becomes clear that prejudice is considered When one’s goals have conflicting implications for stereotype unacceptable. Such increases in the salience of egalitarian norms application, their relative strength may determine whether one can increase the motivation to avoid prejudice and, therefore, applies the stereotype. People who would usually refrain from prompt stereotype inhibition. In one study, norm salience was stereotyping because they are motivated to avoid prejudice may manipulated by doctoring audience reactions to a discussion on nevertheless apply a stereotype if they are sufficiently driven by gay rights (Fein et al., 2003). Participants listened to a discussion self-enhancement goals that can be satisfied by stereotyping (Fein allegedly taped at their school, in which some speakers supported & Spencer, 1997); and people who would usually apply a stereo- and some opposed gay rights. The impression that the audience type to further comprehension may nevertheless refrain from ap- favored either the supporters or the opponents of gay rights was plying it when they become motivated to avoid prejudice created by inserting sounds of applause or silence. Participants (Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000). next received negative feedback on an intelligence test and then, In most of the studies reviewed in this section, stereotype under cognitive load, performed a word-fragment-completion task application was assessed from ratings of target individuals on that assessed their activation of the gay stereotype. During this stereotypic attributes. We also review some studies that assessed task, half the participants were primed with the word gay and half attitudes toward the target from behaviors such as offering the with a neutral word. When people are threatened by failure, a target help (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977) or sitting near the target stereotypic cue can prompt them to activate stereotypes so as to (Snyder, Kleck, Strenta, & Mentzer, 1979) because the application attain their intensified self-enhancement goals (Spencer et al., of attitudes and of beliefs about a group can be governed by the 1998). That is exactly what happened for gay-primed participants same factors. For example, giving people an excuse for seemingly who had been exposed to anti-gay norms. In contrast, gay-primed prejudiced reactions can increase the application of both stereo- participants who had been exposed to pro-gay norms did not types (e.g., Yzerbyt et al., 1994) and attitudes (e.g., Gaertner & activate the gay stereotype; apparently the salient pro-gay norms Dovidio, 1977). increased their motivation to avoid prejudice toward gay people. The inhibitory pressure exerted by this motivation was powerful Motivation to Avoid Prejudice and Stereotype Application enough to override the pressure to activate the stereotype exerted by participants’ self-enhancement goals. The motivation to avoid prejudice can lead perceivers to inhibit In sum, when perceivers interact with a member of a stereotyped the activation of stereotypes applicable to individuals they encoun- group, the extent to which they activate that group’s stereotype ter. This could decrease stereotype application by reducing auto- depends on the amount of attention that they pay to their interac- matic assimilation of the target’s behavior to the stereotype (see tion partner’s group membership as well as on the goals that they Figure 1). But even when a stereotype’s activation has not been are motivated to accomplish during that interaction. Perceivers’ quashed, the motivation to avoid prejudice can decrease its appli- goals may shift as the interaction unfolds, leading to shifts in the cation through a more deliberative process; perceivers can actively extent to which applicable stereotypes are activated. avoid stereotyping others if they have the necessary cognitive resources (Devine, 1989; Fazio & Towles-Schwen, 1999). Several Stereotype Application studies that assessed both stereotype activation and application have found that perceivers may refrain from applying an activated Studies that examined both the activation and the application of group stereotype to the group member whose very presence had stereotypes suggest that on some occasions, perceivers apply ac- provoked its activation (Gilbert & Hixon, 1991, never busy par- STEREOTYPE ACTIVATION AND APPLICATION 533 ticipants; Sinclair & Kunda, 1999, detached observers). Thus, the causes. In recognition of this, contemporary scales of prejudice automatic route from stereotype activation to stereotype applica- such as the Modern Racism Scale (McConahay, 1986) and the tion can be undermined. The motivation to avoid prejudice can Symbolic Racism Scale (Kinder & Sears, 1981) exclude explicit contribute to such undermining. questions that focus directly on beliefs and feelings about nega- Only a handful of studies assessed both the activation and the tively stereotyped groups, and attempt to tap negativity instead application of stereotypes. Most studies reviewed in this section through indirect questions that ask, for example, about attitudes showed that manipulations presumed to affect the motivation to toward busing students to avoid segregation. The underlying as- avoid prejudice can influence stereotype application. Because sumption is that people who would not express explicit negativity these studies did not also assess stereotype activation, we cannot toward a negatively stereotyped group such as African Americans rule out the possibility that their manipulations influenced ste- would nevertheless be willing to express negative views of this reotype application only through their dampening influence on group if these could be attributed to nonracist beliefs. stereotype activation. However, the finding that activated ste- The same holds for reactions to individual group members. reotypes are not always applied suggests that the motivation to People often consider it inappropriate to apply group stereotypes to avoid prejudice can also block the application of activated individuals, but may nevertheless do so if such stereotypic reac- stereotypes. tions can also be attributed to other sources of information. In one Most recent theoretical analyses view the extent to which people study, participants viewed a brief video of a girl whose socioeco- are motivated to avoid prejudice as a stable individual difference nomic background was either high or low (Darley & Gross, 1983). (Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, 1998). We recognize the Participants given only this social-class information did not apply importance of such chronic motives, but also emphasize that it to their judgments of the girl’s intelligence, but participants contextual factors can influence the extent to which people are shown an additional video of the girl performing ambiguously on motivated to avoid prejudice when judging others. Contextual an intelligence test did; they judged her as more intelligent if her influences have been central to the theory of aversive racism social class was high than if it was low. The test performance (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986), which assumes that people become provided an excuse for using a stereotype whose use would oth- less motivated to avoid seemingly prejudiced actions in situations erwise seem inappropriate—stereotype-based judgments could that allow for alternative, nonprejudiced interpretations of these now be attributed to a more legitimate source of information, the same actions. We describe a variety of additional contextual fac- girl’s performance. tors that can affect the motivation to avoid prejudice and, thereby, This possibility gains support from a different study in which influence stereotype application. participants received only an illusion of having individuating in- It is usually quite clear that these contextual factors affect the formation (Yzerbyt et al., 1994). Participants first listened to an motivation to control prejudice, but less clear whether they act on uninformative interview with a person identified at the end as an internally driven motivation to uphold one’s own antiprejudice either a librarian or a comedian. They then had to shadow a voice standards, act on an externally driven motivation to comply with heard in one ear while a different voice was being heard in the others’ antiprejudice standards, or act on both. In most cases, both other ear. Half the participants were later informed (falsely) that seem plausible. For example, the finding that people are more the unattended voice had communicated individuating information likely to show racial discrimination when they can justify doing so about the target (Yzerbyt et al., 1994). These participants’ illusory in terms of race-unrelated factors implies that people are motivated belief that they had obtained individuating information freed them to avoid being considered prejudiced; they avoid any appearance to rely on stereotypes that they would have otherwise avoided of prejudice unless they are confident that they will not be judged using—they rated the librarian as more introverted than the come- as prejudiced (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). But it is unclear whose dian, whereas participants without this illusory belief did not. judgment they are trying to appease—their own, or that of others; Thus, the actual or illusory presence of individuating information this phenomenon could result from people’s motivation to per- can provide an excuse for relying on stereotypes without appearing suade themselves that they are living up to their own egalitarian to violate antistereotyping norms. standards, from their motivation to convince others who saw their The presence of excuses for discrimination can also facilitate behavior that they are free of prejudice, or from both these mo- discriminatory behavior. One relevant study (Gaertner & Dovidio, tives. Indeed, internally and externally driven motivations to avoid 1977) built on an earlier finding that people who encounter a prejudice may arise in tandem; when people become concerned person in distress are less likely to offer help when they believe that they may not be living up to their own antiprejudice standards, other witnesses are present (Latane & Darley, 1970). Another they may also worry that others might view them as prejudiced (cf. witness can provide a justification for one’s own inaction: One Dunton & Fazio, 1997). For most of the factors influencing mo- may assume that the other witness will handle the problem or, if tivation to avoid prejudice that we discuss, it is difficult to disen- the other fails to act, that the problem is not serious enough to tangle influences on internal and on external sources of this warrant intervention. If the person in distress is Black, the avail- motivation. ability of such excuses may diffuse perceivers’ concern that their Availability of excuses for prejudiced reactions. People can failure to help might be construed as reflecting prejudice. In a hold negative beliefs and feelings toward a stereotyped group and, study based on this logic (Gaertner & Dovidio, 1977), White at the same time, be motivated to avoid prejudice (Gaertner & participants believed that they were listening to another partici- Dovidio, 1986; I. Katz & Hass, 1988). This motivation can cause pant, who was either White or Black, over an intercom (in fact, people to scrutinize their reactions carefully to ensure that these they listened to prerecorded audiotapes). They also believed either actions cannot be construed as prejudiced; they will express neg- that they were the only listener or that there were two additional ativity only when such expressions can also be attributed to other listeners. When the speaker appeared to experience an emergency, 534 KUNDA AND SPENCER participants who believed that they were the only witness were series of studies (Monin & Miller, 2001), participants who were slightly more likely to help the Black than the White victim. In first given an opportunity to reject blatantly sexist statements were marked contrast, participants who believed that there were other subsequently more likely to endorse subtly sexist statements. Peo- witnesses present were considerably less likely to help the Black ple who have established their egalitarian credentials are also more than the White victim; they readily discriminated against a Black willing to apply group stereotypes to individual group members. In person when they did not feel vulnerable to the accusation of one study, participants’ confidence in their egalitarian credentials prejudice because they had an excuse for inaction. This implies was boosted by informing them that a test taken earlier revealed that participants’ failure to show comparable discrimination when that they were low in racism. Others received no feedback about they lacked excuses stemmed from their motivation to avoid being their racism (Spencer & Fein, 2000). Participants who had just or appearing prejudiced. been informed that they were low in racism, and who were Another set of studies showed that people are more likely to act therefore confident that their judgments would not be taken as on their negative feelings toward handicapped persons if they have prejudiced, subsequently judged a Black job candidate more a seemingly legitimate excuse for doing so (Snyder et al., 1979). negatively than did controls, and were less likely to prefer him Participants were to evaluate a movie screened on one of two to a stronger White candidate. Confident in their egalitarian monitors. One person was already seated in front of each monitor; credentials, these participants relaxed their efforts to avoid one of these was handicapped and the other was not, so partici- prejudice, and expressed negativity that they would have oth- pants had to choose between sitting next to a handicapped or a erwise curtailed. nonhandicapped person. To manipulate the availability of excuses for avoiding the handicapped person, participants were informed Challenges to impartiality. When one comes under suspicion that the two monitors would show either the same or different of prejudice, one may become especially motivated to ensure that movies. When the monitors were to show different movies, avoid- one’s reactions to stereotyped individuals are unbiased. Therefore, ing the one with the handicapped person could be construed as people who inadvertently behave in a seemingly prejudiced man- reflecting one’s movie preference rather than one’s attitudes to- ner may subsequently go out of their way to prove that they are not ward the handicapped. However, there was no such excuse for prejudiced. Indeed, inducing people to show sexism in one setting avoiding the handicapped person when the two monitors were to can cause them to express less sexism in another setting (Mos- show the same movie. Indeed, participants were considerably more kowitz et al., 1999; Sherman & Gorkin, 1980). Challenges to likely to avoid sitting beside the handicapped person when their impartiality can also influence reactions to stereotyped individuals. choice of seats could be construed as reflecting their movie pref- In one study, White participants viewed a videotaped student erence than when it could not. The availability of a good excuse for discussing race-unrelated issues (Fein et al., 2003). In one condi- shunning a handicapped person alleviated participants’ concern tion the student was White. In three other conditions he was Black, that such behavior would be construed as prejudiced, and freed and mentioned either that he had or had not experienced racism on them to act on negative stereotype-based feelings whose expres- campus, or did not mention racism. When a Black student claims sion they would have otherwise curtailed so as to avoid being or to be a victim of campus racism, this challenges the impartiality of appearing prejudiced. any White student on campus. Indeed, participants so challenged In a different jury study, participants judged a Black defendant rated the Black student considerably more positively than did as more guilty than a White defendant if they had a nonracist participants in the other three conditions. Moreover, for partici- excuse for doing so, but not otherwise (Faranda & Gaertner, 1979, pants who had come under suspicion of prejudice, the motivation cited in Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). All participants read weak to avoid prejudice was powerful enough to prompt them to over- prosecution evidence. Some read, in addition, about evidence that correct for possible stereotypic influences—they rated the Black was extremely damaging to the defendant, but that the jury was student who had challenged their impartiality more positively than instructed to ignore. This inadmissible evidence provided a non- they rated the White student. The motivation to avoid prejudice racist excuse for viewing the defendant as guilty. Indeed, partici- can not only undercut stereotype application, it can also reverse it, pants exposed to it applied their negative stereotype to the defen- resulting in counterapplication. dant, whereas participants who had not seen this evidence, and so Sensitive topics. When a conversation with a member of a lacked an excuse for guilty judgments, did not. stereotyped group turns to a topic sensitive to that group, one may The finding that participants in these studies acted on their worry that one might inadvertently let slip comments that might be negative stereotypes when they had an excuse for doing so sug- gests that these same negative stereotypes were also activated in taken as prejudiced. Earlier, we described a study in which par- participants who, lacking such excuses, refrained from acting on ticipants who expected to discuss a topic sensitive to Asian Cana- their stereotypes. Perceivers’ concerns that they might be or appear dians—Canada’s immigration policies—with an Asian Canadian prejudiced can prevent them from applying activated stereotypes activated the Asian stereotype (Hoshino-Browne & Kunda, 2000). that they would most likely apply if these concerns were assuaged. The thoughts that these participants listed in preparation for this Es