Culture and Child Development PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by Deleted User
Ruma Bose and Sanjida Sattar
Tags
Summary
This document explores how cultural contexts significantly shape child development and parenting practices. It examines differences in everyday routines, caregiving styles, and expectations across cultures. The author discusses the concept of a developmental niche, highlighting the interconnectedness of physical surroundings, cultural customs, and parental beliefs in shaping a child's growth. It also examines attachment styles and how they are influenced by cultural values and practices.
Full Transcript
# Culture and Child Development ## Introduction - Increasing contact between cultures has made the concepts of childhood and the child themselves crucial areas of study. - Anthropologists, historians, and cultural child psychologists have attempted to study how children experience life in differ...
# Culture and Child Development ## Introduction - Increasing contact between cultures has made the concepts of childhood and the child themselves crucial areas of study. - Anthropologists, historians, and cultural child psychologists have attempted to study how children experience life in different contexts. - It has been observed that there are wide differences in how childhoods unfold in different cultures. - This raises the question of how to account for ethnic differences in a way that acknowledges the complexity of social and political contexts, as well as the ways variations in parenting might be an adaptation to different conditions rather than inherent "ethnic differences." ## Developmental Niche and Ecocultural Pathways - The concept of a developmental niche helps frame the factors involved in child development. - It is understood that a child's well-being is shaped by the combination of their physical and social settings, cultural customs related to their care, and parental beliefs about their development. - Everyday routines for the child, which are culturally prescribed, are key to shaping how children learn and grow. - In contexts where child survival is a primary concern, close physical contact and immediate caregiving are prioritized. - This can involve carrying babies, co-sleeping, breastfeeding, and quick responses to crying. - Children's cognitive development may be delayed until survival is assured. - In technologically advanced societies, child survival is less of a concern. - Instead, acquiring skills necessary for success in a knowledge-based economy, particularly language skills, takes center stage. - Mothers in these contexts emphasize verbal interactions, object naming, and teaching language skills from an early age. ## Childhood and Parenting across Cultures - Many conceptualizations of childhood and child development are based on North American and European perspectives, so the typical description of childhood is often derived from these cultural contexts. - It is important to note that even within the Northern hemisphere, there is considerable variation across different cultures and even within those cultures. - While all cultures aim for children to grow into competent adults, specific goals and pathways for achieving those goals vary widely. ### Culture and Child Development - The emphasis on "cultural universality" has been challenged by recent research on child development, which has highlighted the central role of culture in shaping the behavior of both parents and children. - The human mind, while having a universal core, expresses itself through diverse "mentalities" shaped by the historical experiences of different cultures. - This means that development can't be viewed simply as variations from a single, normative standard, but as a reflection of the diverse ways that cultures construct childhood. - This raises the question of whether there is "one childhood," or many unique and culturally shaped forms of childhood. ## Infancy - Infancy is understood as a developmental stage that ends when a child begins to walk and talk, typically around age 2. - It is important to note that this is based on cultural assumptions about the markers of development, rather than on any biological fact. - Some cultures instead mark the beginning of the next stage by the development of a sense of morality, while others emphasize active teaching, beginning as early as age 5. - For example, in the Baganda culture of Uganda, infants are actively encouraged to sit up independently and smile, as these behaviors are highly valued within the culture. ## Attachment - Cross-cultural research on attachment behavior raises questions about whether universal models of development fully capture the complexity of human relationships. - The Bowlby-Ainsworth model proposes a universal model of attachment, based on the caregiver's sensitivity to an infant's signals. - Despite evidence for the universality of attachment behavior across cultures, this model doesn't explicitly account for cultural variation, framing any deviation from the "typical" pattern as sub-optimal or pathological. - Research suggests that diverse cultural contexts might cultivate different "optimal" patterns of attachment, reflecting different interpretations of what constitutes a healthy and desirable attachment. - For example, a study in Bielefeld, Germany, found that 49% of infants exhibited "anxious-avoidant" attachment behavior. - This was attributed to the cultural emphasis on self-reliance and independence, beginning in infancy, and the practice of leaving infants alone to encourage their self-sufficiency. - While there is no higher incidence of personality disorders in this community, this example illustrates how cultural values can shape attachment behavior in ways not accounted for by universal models. - Another example comes from both British and American middle-class culture up to the end of World War II, where a strong emphasis on preventing infant dependency was highly valued. - Additionally, the Efe culture of Zaire demonstrates the diversity in infant care-taking patterns. - Multiple caretakers, beyond the mother, are involved in raising infants, and this kind of care-giving is normalized in their society. ## Culture and Child Development - Tools like diagnostic categories, the accessibility of services, and instruments of measurement are all shaped by cultural influences, making it challenging to draw definitive conclusions about cultural differences in mental health globally. - Cross-cultural epidemiological studies have not yet yielded conclusive evidence for "culture-specific syndromes," but it is important to note the influence of cultural factors on the prevalence of certain disorders. - For instance, there is evidence that dissociative disorders such as trance and possession in adolescents might be linked to rapid social change in cultures where such beliefs are prevalent.. - Further, a review of the mental health of children from main ethnic groups in Britain found that, despite socioeconomic challenges faced by Pakistani, Bangladeshi, and Afro-Caribbean children in the UK, these individuals show comparable or even better mental health compared to the White population. - This underscores the need to investigate the interplay of risk and protective factors for different communities, highlighting the importance of context-specific care. ## Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder - A systematic review of ADHD estimates that the worldwide pooled prevalence is 5.29%. - However, it is important to note that most studies are from North America and Europe, and prevalence ranges from 1% to 20% across different studies. - Ongoing challenges in standardization and interpretation across countries raise questions about how cultural factors might influence the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD, and whether the differences in prevalence rates reflect different cultural thresholds for accepting behavior that might be considered non-conforming. ## Eating Disorders - There is evidence for a significant role of cultural influence in the etiology of eating disorders, as rates vary widely worldwide. - Rising rates, potentially attributed to cultural changes like modernization and urbanization, have been observed. - For example, research conducted in the Netherlands shows a five-fold increase in bulimia from 1985 to 1999, with urbanization as a contributing factor. ## Self-Harm - In the UK, a review of non-fatal self-harm has revealed a higher rate among South Asian girls. - While cultural conflict has been proposed as a potential explanation, it is important to consider other contributing factors. - Research has indicated that migrant parents may have a higher perception of risk for their children in their new environment, which might lead to stricter parental control and potentially increase the risk of conflict between generations. - The act of self-harm, in this context, might be a culturally shaped way of expressing distress for young South Asian girls in the UK. ## Conclusion - There is no single, definitive answer to the question of how culture shapes childhood, as both culture and ethnicity are dynamic and constantly evolving. - It is important to move beyond "cultural differences" and acknowledge the centrality of culture in understanding the development of children. - This involves recognizing the importance of incorporating the various cultural influences shaping the lives of children and understanding how they come to be seen as "different." - Mental health professionals, especially those working with children from diverse cultures, need to be aware of the potent role of culture in shaping children's development and to work towards promoting reflexivity in their work by including knowledge about the everyday lives of children from various parts of the world. - Moving away from a singular, Eurocentric view of childhood is vital for providing greater understanding and effective interventions.