Life Course Theory PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by ChasteIvory2510
Columbia College
2015
Glen H. Elder Jr., Michael J. Shanahan, and Julia A. Jennings
Tags
Related
- PUB 460 Lecture 4 - Life Course Perspective On MCH - II PDF
- Resumen Examen Licenciatura - Anto M 2023 - PDF
- Developmental Psychology PDF - PCU B.Sc. 1st Year (2023)
- Lesson 03 Introduction to Life Course and Its Theories (Behavioral Sciences and Ethics) BIU 2024 PDF
- Life Course Stages and Health Development PDF
- PSYC 474 Midterm PDF
Summary
This document is an excerpt from 'Life Course Theory', focusing on human development within social contexts. It discusses the influence of historical time and place on development using examples of social change in China and Eastern Europe.
Full Transcript
CHAPTER 2 Human Development in Time and Place GLEN H. ELDER JR., MICHAEL J. SHANAHAN, and JULIA A. JENNINGS INTRODUCTION 1 LIVES AND CONTEXT: HUMAN AGENCY THE DEVELOPMENT OF LIFE COURSE THEORY 2 AND SO...
CHAPTER 2 Human Development in Time and Place GLEN H. ELDER JR., MICHAEL J. SHANAHAN, and JULIA A. JENNINGS INTRODUCTION 1 LIVES AND CONTEXT: HUMAN AGENCY THE DEVELOPMENT OF LIFE COURSE THEORY 2 AND SOCIAL OPTIONS 28 Bringing Contexts and Temporality to Lives and Context and the Life Course 28 Development 5 Conceptualization and Measurement 29 Life-Span Concepts of Human Development 9 Selection and the Life Course: A Social Process 32 Social Relations: Roles and Sequences 10 THE IMPACT OF HISTORICAL TIME AND PLACE 35 Age and the Life Course 12 Studying Lives in Context: Some Considerations 35 Converging Research Traditions in Life Course Theory 14 Social Change in Life Course Health: The Case of ELEMENTARY LIFE COURSE CONCEPTS China 37 AND PERSPECTIVES 14 Societal Dissolution and Unification: Their Impact Social Pathways, Cumulative Processes, and Durations 15 on Young Lives 38 Trajectories, Transitions, and Turning Points 19 HUMAN DEVELOPMENT IN CONTEXT 42 Social Change and Life Transitions 21 REFERENCES 43 Linking Mechanisms 21 Paradigmatic Principles 23 INTRODUCTION The term context refers to the social embedding of individ- uals and typically entails study of biographical, historical, The life course and human development has flourished as and ecological variations. The social concept of life course a field of study during the past quarter century, extending refers to a temporal pattern of age-graded events and roles across substantive and theoretical boundaries (Mortimer that chart the social contours of biography, providing a & Shanahan, 2003), and now appears in many subfields proximal context for the dynamics of human development of the social, behavioral, and medical sciences. With this from conception and birth to death. change has come an increasing appreciation for link- Conceptual and methodological breakthroughs asso- ages between changing contexts and human development. ciated with the interdisciplinary life course framework, coupled with the dramatic expansion of long-term longitu- dinal studies, have generated more research and knowledge We thank Ross Parke, Avshalom Caspi, and Richard Lerner for than ever before about behavioral adaptations in real- thoughtful reviews of the earliest version of this chapter (Elder, world settings around the globe. We are also increasingly 1998a) and to Lilly Shanahan for her valuable review of the second aware of people as agents of their own lives. New avenues version (Elder & Shanahan, 2006). Rainer Silbereisen provided a most helpful review of the present version. Our special thanks of research have opened, and the future offers exciting to the staff of the Carolina Population Center for preparation of promise for understanding how dynamic views of con- the first two versions of the chapter under a grant from Eunice text and the person—including biological dimensions— Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human interact to influence achievements, exposure to stres- Development (NIH/NRSA T32 HD07168) and to Terry Poythress sors, physical and psychological well-being, and social for her preparation of this version. involvements. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 7th Edition, edited by Richard M. Lerner, Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 1 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 2 Human Development in Time and Place This contextualization of lives and developmental pro- work and the theoretical implications of research on social cesses occurs through the patterning of social roles, events, contexts and the flow of families and children between and age distinctions; and in a multilevel context of fam- them. Lives are lived by entering and leaving social roles, ily/primary group, neighborhood, community, economic groups, and places. What factors influence these decisions? region, and country. The meaning of historical time and How can we understand human agency and contextual context stems in large part from the ecological process effects as parents construct the residential life course of of place and its multiple levels (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). their children? We investigate such questions through A distinctive feature of this ecology is its social inequalities studies of place and migration in the lives of families and of class, ethnicity, and gender. They are expressed across children. Genetic dispositions are relevant to this process, individual lives and the generations in cumulative dynam- and we refer readers to our prior edition of this chapter ics of advantage and disadvantage through childhood, (Elder & Shanahan, 2006) for such coverage. adolescence, and the adult years. Ecological influences are expressed in part through the We begin this chapter by viewing the evolution of life impact of their historical time on lives and developmental course thinking as a response to the challenges that stem processes. Although studies have tended to consider eco- from following children into young adulthood, middle logical effects apart from historical context, we attempt to age, and late life. This chapter is also a product of the inform this section of the chapter with both perspectives. remarkable growth of these studies from the 1960s to the Three topics highlight their interdependence: (1) consid- end of the century. Life course ideas in developmental erations in studying changing times in lives; (2) societal science, social roles and relationships, and concepts of the change in lives, with a focus on contemporary China and its age-graded life course are prominent in this conceptual rural–urban divide; and (3) the impact of social discontinu- advance. By the end of the 1990s, a new synthesis, linking ities on the life course of young people during the dissolu- theory on social relationships and age, had become a tion of the Soviet Union into multiple sovereign states (late theoretical orientation on the social life course and its 1980s) and the reunification of Germany (1991). These two influences on human development in historical and ecolog- events transformed life in Eastern Europe, especially for the ically defined contexts. Multiple lives are interdependent young who faced a new world of opportunities and stresses. in this developmental process. We conclude this chapter by noting that the contextual fron- The elementary concepts and perspectives of life-course tier on human development is moving toward an integration theory are surveyed next, with emphasis on the individual of ecological and temporal perspectives. life course, its institutionalized pathways, and its social and The title of this chapter reflects its intergenerational, life developmental trajectories and transitions. Early research course, and longitudinal perspective. Longitudinal samples on social change in lives has generated a set of mecha- enable us to follow children into adolescence and then to nisms that link lives and developmental dynamics to chang- young adulthood with its social roles of advanced educa- ing contexts. These mechanisms include the life stage of tion, military service, parenthood, and work. According people when they encounter drastic change to their envi- to this developmental life course perspective, children age ronment, the social imperatives that structure adaptations into adulthood and its family roles, and parents eventually to new situations, the control cycle that life change initi- become grandparents. At any point in the life span, all ates (loss of personal control prompts efforts to regain such ages are commonly represented in a person’s social world. control), and the tendency for new situations to accentuate The developmental significance of early life experience matching dispositions. These mechanisms are embedded becomes most fully understood in the context of the in a conceptual framework on the life course and develop- later years. ment that is defined by core paradigmatic principles—the life-long process of human development and aging, the tim- ing of events in the life course, human agency, the interde- THE DEVELOPMENT OF LIFE COURSE THEORY pendence of lives, and historical time and place. We discuss these mechanisms and principles by drawing on relevant The magnitude of intellectual development in life course theory and research. studies is suggested by considering studies of person and Traditional thinking about the place or location of society during the 1950s. In his widely read The Socio- individuals is undergoing significant elaboration through logical Imagination, C. Wright Mills (1959) encouraged ecological studies of human development. We turn to this “the study of biography, of history, and of the problems 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 3 of their intersection within social structure” (p. 149). Mills 1950s, and another follow-up, scheduled in 1972 to 1973, started with the individual and asked what features of soci- joined the lives of all study members, some parents, and ety produce such a person. He argued that the seemingly offspring, in an intergenerational framework. The Berke- “personal problems” of one’s biography are better under- ley Guidance and Growth Studies became part of this stood as repercussions of broad social tensions. He had few follow-up. By the 1970s, Block (with the assistance of empirical examples, however, and was not concerned with Haan; see Block & Haan, 1971), had completed a pioneer- dynamic views of person and context. Rather, he focused ing longitudinal study focused on continuity and change on types of society and adult behavioral patterns, with lit- in personality from early adolescence to the middle years tle recognition of social change, development and aging, in the lives of the Oakland and Berkeley study members. or even human diversity. In this age of the cross-sectional Also during the 1970s, Vaillant (1977) followed a panel survey, studies that followed children and adults over part of Harvard men (recruited as students between 1939 and of their lives were very rare. This was especially true for 1942, known as the W. T. Grant Study) into the middle longitudinal studies of people in their social and histori- years of adulthood, assessing mechanisms of defense cal contexts. With this in mind, it is not surprising that a and coping. dynamic concept of the life course had not yet appeared in Another study at the Institute of Human Development the scholarly literature and was not addressed in the semi- (Elder, 1974/1999) placed the lives of members of the nars of leading graduate programs. Oakland Growth Study and Berkeley Guidance Study in The unfolding story of life course theory up to the the Great Depression and traced the influence of hardship present owes much to path-breaking studies that were on family life, careers, and health up to midlife. Using launched more than 80 years ago at the Institute of Child data from a retrospective life history survey, this study Welfare (now Human Development) at the University of also investigated the impact of military service in World California in Berkeley: The Oakland Growth Study (birth War II and the Korean War on men’s lives. To cap off years 1920 to 1921) and the Berkeley Growth and Guid- this active decade, investigators at the institute conducted ance Studies (birth years 1928 to 1929). These studies were a multifaceted study that revealed patterns of continuity launched around 1930–1931. When the studies began, no and change in social roles, health, and personality, with one could have imagined what they eventually would a distinctive emphasis on life patterns across the middle mean for the field of human development. The original years (Eichorn, Clausen, Haan, Honzik, & Mussen, 1981). investigators did not envision research that extended into Both historical cohort comparisons and intergenerational the study members’ adult years, let alone into the later connections were part of this project. years of middle and old age. At Stanford University, a research team headed by There were many reasons for this focus on childhood Robert Sears actively followed members of the Lewis and adolescence. Except for support from the Laura Terman sample of talented children into their later years. Spelman Rockefeller Foundation, funds for longitudinal The Terman Study had become the oldest, active longitu- studies were virtually nonexistent. The National Institutes dinal study at the time, with birth years extending from of Health (NIH), major funders of such studies today, 1903 to the 1920s. By the 1990s, the project had assembled were not established until after World War ll. With support 13 waves of data spanning 70 years (Holahan & Sears, from NIH, the classic Framingham Longitudinal Heart 1995), and research was beginning to show the historical Study of the adult years was launched in 1946 and has imprint of the times on the study members’ lives, from evolved into a multigenerational project. However, the the 1920s to the post–World War II years and into old idea of adult development had not yet captured the atten- age (Crosnoe & Elder, 2004: Shanahan & Elder, 2002). tion of social, behavioral, and medical science. A mature Over 40% of the men entered military service during World field of adult development and aging was still decades War II and 25% were involved in war industries on the away from becoming a reality. In the United States, the home front (Elder, Pavalko, & Clipp, 1993). The lives of National Institute of Aging was not established until the women in the Terman sample vividly reflect the gender-role mid-1970s. constraints of society on their employment. Nonetheless, these barriers did not restrict the studies This extension of the child samples to the adult years from continuing into the adult years and middle age. provided an initial momentum for the scientific study of The Institute of Human Development contacted members adult development and sharpened awareness of the need of the Oakland Growth Study for interviews in the late for a different research paradigm that would pay attention 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 4 Human Development in Time and Place to human development beyond childhood and to contexts To relate lives to an ever-changing society, with empha- beyond the family. Such work offered great promise for sis on the developmental effects of social change and illuminating the intergenerational dynamics of parents and transitions. their children. The extension also enabled documentation of the implications of early childhood experience for As a whole, these challenges represent a view of human health in later life, a research domain of major scientific development advocated by proponents of contextualized significance in the 21st century (see Herd, Robert, & development (e.g., Cairns & Cairns, 2006) and by the early House, 2011). Child-based models of development had Chicago school of sociology (Abbott, 1997), especially little to offer because they did not address development William I. Thomas. In the first decades of the twentieth and aging in the adult life course and were not concerned century, a time of transformation in U.S. society, Thomas with changing social contexts. For the most part, the made a persuasive case for studying social change as Oakland and Berkeley studies of continuity and change “experiments of nature” in the lives of immigrants and from childhood to the adult years were limited to evidence children. Inspired by The Polish Peasant in Europe and of correlational patterns between measures at time l and America (Thomas & Znaniecki, 1918), researchers began time 2 (Jones, Bayley, Macfarlane, & Honzik, 1971). The to use life-record data to investigate the impact of social intervening years and their mechanisms remained a “black change. Before most of the innovative longitudinal studies box.” Little, if anything, could be learned about linking had been launched, Thomas urged in the mid-1920s that events and processes from such analysis. priority be given to “the longitudinal approach to life This observation also applies to Kagan and Moss history” (Volkart, 1951, p. 593). He claimed that studies (1962) who studied children in the Fels Longitudinal should investigate “many types of individuals with regard Study from “birth to maturity” by using correlation coef- to their experiences and various past periods of life in ficients to depict behavioral stability across the years. different situations” and follow “groups of individuals in Their approach ignored the diverse paths children take the future, getting a continuous record of experiences as into adult life. By age 23, some of the study members they occur.” followed a path to college, full-time employment, and Social transformations of the 20th century raised many marriage, and others entered military service or mixed questions about historical variations beyond family life and employment and education. The timing of such transitions kinship, such as schools, neighborhoods, and communities. was important in determining their meaning and implica- In the classic Middletown studies (Lynd & Lynd, 1929, tions. For example, adolescent marriage and parenting are 1937), findings on families during the 1920s seemed to coupled with more social and economic constraints than have little relevance to family life in the Great Depression. the same transitions that follow a normative timetable, Life course theory emerged in response to such issues and whereas late family formation maximizes the disruptive to the challenge of an aging population as well as the rapid effect of young children. However, these considerations of growth of longitudinal studies. In the terminology of this timing and context—so richly descriptive of lives—were chapter, the life course refers most broadly to a theoretical of little interest. In large part, this inattention reflected orientation (or paradigm) that provides a framework for the the view that continuity of behaviors and psychological study of changing lives in changing contexts. To use the dispositions required little explanation aside from the label distinction of Merton (1968), theoretical orientations estab- “stability.” lish a common field of inquiry by defining a framework Empirical studies of children into the adult and midlife that guides research in terms of problem identification years revealed major limitations to conventional knowledge and formulation, variable selection and rationales, and of human development, which, in turn, posed major chal- strategies of research design and analysis. lenges for the future study of behavior: Based in large part on sociocultural theories of age and social relationships (Elder, 1975; Neugarten, 1968; Ryder, To replace child-based, “ontogenetic” accounts of 1965), the concept of life course refers to a sequence of development with models that apply to development age-graded events and roles that defines the sociological and aging over the life course. contours of biography. A sociocultural perspective gives To think about how human lives are organized socially emphasis to the social meanings of age. Birth, puberty, and and develop over time, exhibiting patterns of constancy death are biological facts, but their meanings in the life and change. course are social facts or constructions. Age distinctions 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 5 are expressed in expectations about the timing and order fortunes, residential mobility, and neighborhood compo- of a transition or change in state, whether relatively early, sition (Shanahan, Sulloway, & Hofer, 2000). Each life is on time, or late. The life course can be linked historically marked by social change in these respects, and the life to specific transitions and to the meanings of cohort mem- course framework is useful in studying how these dynamics bership (Riley, Johnson, & Foner, 1972). Birth year locates shape lives and also how the social aggregate of individual people in specific birth cohorts and thus according to par- life patterns affect social institutions, such as schools and ticular social changes. The social life course of individuals labor markets. is embedded within specific birth cohorts and their eco- logical dynamics. These dynamics may take the form of Bringing Contexts and Temporality cumulative processes of life course inequality. to Lives and Development These dynamics may be expressed as cumulative pro- cesses of social inequality from early childhood into the The socioeconomic context of human development became adult life course. Disparities in socioeconomic status, eth- a compelling social issue in the hard times of the Great nicity, and gender can initiate processes of disadvantage Depression (1930s), but the economic crisis did not or advantage that increasingly differentiate people over the place this theme on the research agenda of the California life course. There are numerous scenarios of cumulative longitudinal studies, the Oakland and Berkeley projects disadvantage, such as the early death of a parent, which (see Duncan, Magnusson, & Votruba-Drzal, Chapter 14, results in a child’s depressed feelings, behavior problems in this Handbook, this volume). They continued to reflect the school, erratic attendance, and the eventual loss of opportu- research interests of the investigators rather than the eco- nity. Potential turning points along this life course can lib- nomic depression. The Oakland Growth Study focused on erate youth from the grip of this negative dynamic such as physical growth and development, a long-time interest of through residential change that improves family life and the a codirector, and employed methods of social observation school environment (Wachs, Chapter 21, this Handbook, in field settings. The Berkeley Study under Jean Macfar- this volume). lane’s leadership stressed the collection of data on family G. H. Elder, this chapter’s senior author, encountered relationships and parental influences. Data collection for such ideas about age and the life course in the 1960s, both projects included information on the socioeconomics shortly after arriving at the Institute of Human Develop- of family life, but the investigators did not make effective ment (at UC Berkeley in 1962) to work with sociologist use of the data in empirical research. It would be diffi- J. A. Clausen on the Oakland Growth Study. The dra- cult to know from study publications that the Oakland matic changes of families and individual lives across the and Berkeley children were growing up during the Great 1930s focused his attention on the patterning of lives and Depression. connections to a changing socioeconomic environment. The absence of a socioeconomic-cultural context Codes that captured trajectories were needed for peo- beyond the immediate family in the Berkeley Study was ple’s lives instead of the conventional codes for status noted by a faculty member whom Macfarlane had invited at a point in time such as socioeconomic status (SES). to one of the study’s seminars. In a letter dated September The link between age and time provided an important step 25, 1941, this person (identity unknown) expressed dismay in this direction. The resulting perspective suggested a concerning the neglect of material culture. In his view, way of thinking about the social construction of individual family was overemphasized at the expense of other cultural lives, along with ideas from the life-history tradition of factors. With reference to the case of a young girl in the the early Chicago School of Sociology. Children of the study, he observed that “she is described as a person of Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999) represented the pub- almost any age in almost any society.” Despite an inade- lished version of this initial effort to fashion a life course quate contextualization of development, the early Berkeley framework. and Oakland studies made sure that measures of the mate- Since its inception, the field of life course studies rial culture were used in data collection across the 1930s has expanded its purview beyond historical variations and thus ensured that these data would be available to sub- to include dynamic contextual variations within and sequent generations of investigators. As a result, the senior between cohorts—the ecology of human development author was able to carry out a longitudinal study of “chil- (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Studies revealed dramatic cohort dren growing up in the Great Depression.” The Oakland diversity with respect to poverty experiences and economic data archive included socioeconomic information for 1929 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 6 Human Development in Time and Place (before family income change) and 1933, the very worst 1978, 1982; Modell, 1989), and anthropology (Kertzer year of the economic depression. & Keith, 1984), to ecological models (Bronfenbrenner, This Great Depression project evolved from the senior 1979) and life-span developmental psychology (Baltes & author’s research affiliation with the Oakland study at the Nesselroade, 1979). Major examples include: Institute of Human Development in the 1960s. Trained Recognition of a life course perspective on human devel- in both sociology and psychology, Elder had been hired opment that extends from the prenatal period to maturity, by the new director of the institute, sociologist Clausen, late life, and death. The rapid growth of longitudinal studies to work toward a design for coding the Oakland data. that link childhood to the adaptations of later life has facil- The ever-changing families of the Oakland Study sensi- itated what might be called a “whole life course” approach tized Elder to the need for temporal concepts and measures to human development and aging (Elder & Giele, 2009). and focused his attention on “ways of thinking about An understanding of the trajectory of human development social change, life pathways, and individual development” and aging begins in the prenatal years. This observation is a (Elder, 1998b, p. 1). But how to conceptualize them? His foundational theme of the Millennium National Longitudi- prior work on adolescence and the transition to adulthood nal Study in the United Kingdom. The project was launched introduced him to the research of Neugarten (1968) on the during 2000 and 2001 as a study of how the British people meanings of age and age-graded expectations and timeta- age from birth to old age and death. bles. This anthology includes Neugarten’s pioneering papers from the 1950s and early 1960s. Other age concepts Life-history calendars for the collection of retrospective on historical time and timing were associated with birth accounts of life events have been applied to numerous year and age cohorts, as developed by Ryder (1965). longitudinal studies (Brückner & Mayer, 1998; Caspi Role theory and the social capital of linked lives pro- et al., 1996; Freedman, Thornton, Camburn, Alwin, vided another way to think of the life course and its rela- & Young-DeMarco, 1988). Retrospective life history tion to other lives. The concept of role transitions by life methods enable investigators to collect information stage indicates whether the transitions are early or later in on the life history of people and their world, though a person’s life. Roles and their behavior could be viewed in retrospection always entails some error of recall. terms of experiences that are brought to them and in terms Greater appreciation for the necessity of longitu- of the time span of “being in that social role,” as well as dinal and contextually rich data (Ferri, Bynner, & according to issues of continuity and discontinuity associ- Wadsworth, 2003; Hauser, 2009; Phelps, Furstenberg, ated with leaving a role. Along with the traditions of life & Colby, 2002). In a special issue of Science, Butz and history and career studies, the concept of life cycle was per- Torrey (2006) refer to the longitudinal study design as haps the most prominent perspective on a person’s life at one of the greatest innovations of the 20th century in the time, especially regarding family life. In a life cycle of the social sciences—“a living observatory and potential generational succession, the young person is socialized to laboratory augmented by case study and ethnography.” maturity, gives birth and nurtures members of the next gen- Bynner (2014) describes the longitudinal survey as eration, grows old, and dies. Each concept has relevance to “the essential tool for meeting the challenges of a a person’s life path. Role theory, as well as the life cycle, (developmental) science that needs to adapt continually became part of an effort at the Institute of Human Develop- in response to social, economic, technological, and ment to develop a theoretical approach to individual lives political change.” and human development that would be useful for a study of Appropriate statistical techniques have been developed the Oakland cohort across the Great Depression. With fam- for multilevel, longitudinal studies. They include hierar- ily income available for 1929 and 1933, the Oakland study chical linear and trajectory models along with structural could assess the extent of socioeconomic deprivation and and dynamic person-variable and person-centered tech- its consequences among families and the study members. niques (Bergman, Magnusson, & El-Khouri, 2003; This approach to lives in changing times and places has Collins & Sayer, 2001; Little, Schnabel, & Baumert, evolved into a prominent theoretical orientation on the life 2000). Significant advances have also been made in course in the twenty-first century. Notable developments the study of historical and cohort effects through new have occurred across the social and behavioral sciences, age-period-cohort methods that provide better estimates from sociology (Elder, 1974/1999, 1975, 1985; Riley and identify explanatory mechanisms (Yang & Land, et al., 1972), demography (Ryder, 1965), history (Hareven, 2013). The past two decades have also witnessed major 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 7 advances in the study of “the ecology” of human devel- These connections with life course theory and research opment. Sampson (2012) has used the term ecometrics add up to a much larger intellectual advance, one framed by to refer to social observational methods in studying relational developmental systems thinking in a multilevel, urban and rural places (see also Wachs, Chapter 21, this dynamic perspective known as developmental science. Handbook, this volume). From the 1998 edition of the Handbook of Child Psychol- Cross-disciplinary models of collaboration, particularly ogy and in the sixth edition, Lerner (2006, p. 6) observed with psychology and history as well as biology and that students of human development have witnessed “a sea the medical sciences (Elder, Modell, & Parke, 1993; change that perhaps qualifies as a true paradigm shift in Levy & the Pavie Team, 2005). These models include what is thought of as the nature of human nature and in new and exciting developments in subfields devoted the appreciation of time, place, and individual diversity for to the study of physical and emotional well-being understanding the laws of human behavior and develop- (Halfon & Hochstein, 2002; Hertzman & Power, 2003; ment.” Consistent with the central theme of this chapter, Kuh, Ben-Shlomo, Lynch, Hallqvist, & Power, 2003). Lerner asserted that “one must appreciate how variables New initiatives from the Maternal and Child Health associated with person, place, and time coalesce to shape Bureau emphasize the life course perspective, such the structure and function of behavior and its systematic as the formation of a Maternal and Child Health Life and successive change” (2006, p. 7). Course Research Network. The objective of the network The principal traditions that led to an interdisci- is to facilitate life course studies that inform Maternal plinary framework of life course theory are illustrated and Child Health programs, policy, and practice and by Figure 2.1: life-span development, social roles and improve “health outcomes for mothers and children.” relationships, and age and temporality. We begin with A growing awareness that, beyond history and the life-span concepts of development because this line of differing experiences of cohorts, social change may work prompted efforts to contextualize developmental entail an ecological change within cohorts through processes across the life span. Two theoretical traditions in diverse life histories (Shanahan, Mortimer, & Kruger, social science, social roles/relationships and age, provide a 2002). Aspects of a social ecology are typically inter- way to think about the social life course. Social roles and correlated, and their synergistic interactions are critical role transitions are basic elements of the life course, but to an understanding of time and place. they are timeless. That is, a role transition is not specific in terms of when it occurs. Chronological age brings time These developments represent significant advances and timing to the social life course, and thus makes it more in studies of the life course and human development. dynamic as a contextualization of development. Age data Life course theory today has much in common with inter- on birth year also locate individuals in historical time and actionist thinking at the micro level, with its emphasis on in relation to ecological processes. transactions between person and ecology (see Magnusson Life-span development refers in some ways to the & Stattin, 2006)—but it also attends to the organizations longitudinal research that was underway at the Institute of and reorganization of social structures and pathways Human Development when the senior author joined the through life. As might be expected, life course theory staff to work with the Oakland Growth Study (in 1962). shares many objectives and concepts with Bronfenbren- Bayley and Honzik were involved in longitudinal stud- ner’s ecology of human development (1979; with Morris, ies of intellectual development from childhood into the 2006), especially its multilevel concept of the environ- adult years (Jones et al., 1971). Other longitudinal studies ment. However, life course models bring a more temporal focused on the stability of temperament dimensions from perspective to the environment and individual. The life the early years into adulthood. Block had launched a course paradigm also shares the ambition of life-span program of research that used the California Q Sort to developmental psychology in rethinking the nature of assess personality in adolescence and the adult years for a human development and aging (Baltes, Lindenburg, & longitudinal study of life-span trajectories of personality. Staudinger, 2006), but it is more contextual in orientation This project became Lives Through Time (Block & Haan, and application. Indeed, the contextual limitations of 1971). In method, most especially, this ambitious study the Oakland and Berkeley life-span studies in the early represents a path-breaking example of a person-centered 1960s motivated efforts to place lives and developmental study of life-span trajectories of personality. However, processes in historical time and social pathways. this project, as well as others noted earlier, was seriously 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 8 Human Development in Time and Place Life-Span Concepts Social Roles and of Development Relationships Age and Temporality Psychosocial stage, Life cycle of social roles, Age-grades, expectations, adult stages of development generational succession concepts of age status identity, proscriptive and prescriptive Developmental trajectories Social roles, status, age norms role-playing Cumulative advantage, Social pathways and trajectories disadvantage Role transitions and sequences Transitions and turning points Selective optimization Socialization as learning with compensation Cohorts - Birth cohorts Intergenerational relations, and social change, structural lag Life review, exchange autobiographical memory Social networks, capital Person-Context Interaction Life Course Theory 1960s to present Figure 2.1 The emergence of life course theory: Research traditions and their concepts. underdeveloped on the contextual side. None of them In the theoretical tradition of “social relations,” we provided an understanding of the lived lives of the study come to a long prominent way of thinking about a person’s members in historical time. lived life, with a focus on the sequence of social roles, their Pioneering work under the theme of life-span concepts socialization, and self or identity. The sequence establishes features the studies and writings of Erikson (1950) on ego a life course that links the person to others. Central to this identity and psychosocial stages of development as well as tradition is Merton (1968) on role sets and reference groups, the foundational contributions of Baltes (1997) to the evo- Rosenberg (1979) on self-esteem, and Bronfenbrenner lution of life-span developmental psychology, from the late (1970) on socialization, to name a few. Early work in 1960s into the 21st century. This contribution includes his this tradition includes the studies of Thomas (Thomas & conceptualization of the process of selective optimization Znaniecki, 1918) on social roles and transitions in life his- with compensation, a metatheory of development and aging tories, Mead (1934) on socialization and the self, Hughes discussed more fully in the pages to come. In a younger (1971) on work and the self, Lewin (1948) on power- generation, Lerner (1982, 1991) emphasized the relative dependence relations, and Vygotsky (1978) on language, plasticity and agency of the organism, the multidirection- the self, and social relationships. ality of life-span development, and the lifelong interaction Studies of intergenerational relations have expanded of person and social context. The concept of developmen- from two to three and now even four generations, with tal task, perhaps first defined by Havighurst (1949), also important contributions from Jackson (2000) and his represented a way of viewing development across socially three-generation study of African Americans along with a defined life stages. The concept alerts the analyst to the rural Iowa longitudinal study of three generations (Elder & possibility that different experiences and skills tend to be Conger, 2000). The most impressive multigeneration lon- highly salient at different points in life. However, empirical gitudinal study to date was initiated by Bengtson circa evidence for distinct psychosocial stages is not compelling. 1970 (Bengston, Putney, & Harris, 2013) on contemporary The perceived or defined life course can change with aging issues of the generation gap. Launched in the greater Los through successive life reviews (Staudinger, 1989) in which Angeles region, this study has continued into the present the past is assessed in light of the present. century with four, and even five, living generations. 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 9 Several topics illustrate distinctive contributions to the time and place (Mitterauer, 1993). In The Seasons of a third strand of life course theory—age and temporality. Man’s Life, Levinson (1978) outlined a theory of life struc- Every event in life is marked by an age, such as marriage ture that ignored variations in social structure and culture and the birth of children. Birthday celebrations mark each over historical time. Psychosocial transitions were affixed new year for a young child who is surrounded by adults to age as if immutable to institutional change, such as the who are getting older. In the first volume of the Annual midlife transition between ages 40 and 45. For Erikson, Review of Sociology (Elder, 1975), the senior author’s Levinson, and other ontogenetic theorists, the starting essay focused on two life course perspectives based on point is a sequence of stages through which all persons age, the role of age and birth year in a cohort historical must pass. This perspective views the social context as perspective, and a sociocultural perspective involving age a “scene or setting” through which the person—loaded expectations, identities, and norms. Social and cultural with his or her “natural predispositions”—must pass. anthropologists, such as Mead (1963) and Linton (1942), By contrast, the life course paradigm views the interplay of observed and wrote about the role of age-graded societies social context and the organism as the formative process, and lives. Early contributions to the scholarship of age making people who they are. Individuals do not “develop in the 1920s appear on the cohort level, as in the work according to their natures” but, rather, they are continually of Mannheim (1952). Age as birth year locates people as produced, sustained, and changed by their social context members of a birth cohort in social history. (see Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter, 2006). During the 1950s and 1960s, Neugarten (1968, 1996) Proponents of life-span developmental science ad- at the University of Chicago’s Committee on Human dressed the challenges of such a view by seeking a per- Development developed a social psychology of age across spective on development and aging across the life span the life span. She explored concepts of age expectations that emphasized cultural influences and learned experi- and identities, standards, and norms in pioneering studies ences or skills in patterns of aging. In theory, historical during the 1950s. In the 1970s, sociologist Riley (Riley and cultural variations emerge as potentially influential et al., 1972) proposed a framework on age strata and sources of human adaptation and development. As Baltes cohorts for a macroscopic perspective on aging, drawing (1979, p. 265) observed, “restricting developmental events on Ryder’s influential perspective (1965) regarding cohorts to those which have the features of a biological growth in the study of social change. A cohort perspective based on concept of development is more of a hindrance than a help.” people born in a particular year or a specific historical time Baltes (1993, 1994) played a lead role in the con- soon began to appear with some frequency, as in studies ceptual articulation of life-span development since the of women’s work by Uhlenberg (1974), research on role 1960s. More than most proponents of this perspective, sequences in the transition to adulthood by Hogan (1981), he interacted with life course ideas and distinctions over and Birth and Fortune, a volume by Easterlin (1987). In the decades (see Baltes et al., 2006). One panel exchange the field of social history, accounts of institutional and between Baltes and Elder on life-span developmental cultural change brought historical insights to the lives psychology and life course theory was held at the 2004 and pathways of young people (Modell, 1989) and adults Ghent meeting of the International Society of Behavioral (Hareven, 1978, 1982). With these brief overviews in mind, Development. we turn to the development of life course theory, beginning The following propositions on life-span development with life-span concepts of development. are not new in themselves but they add up to a distinctive perspective: Life-Span Concepts of Human Development Life-span development results from lifelong adaptive A number of efforts in the psychological sciences have been processes in which some are cumulative and continuous, made during the post–World War II era to link developmen- and others are discontinuous and innovative, showing tal trajectories to social structure. However, research ques- little connection to prior events or processes. tions did not ask about the implications of environmental Ontogenetic development is local, specific, and time change for the developing individual. bound, so it is never fully adaptive. There is no pure The theory of psychosocial stages formulated by advance or loss in development. Erikson (1950) paid attention to cultural variations, but Age-graded influences are most important in the depen- historians report little empirical support of his stages across dency years, childhood/adolescence and old age, but 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 10 Human Development in Time and Place history-graded and nonnormative influences are most influences shape development in different ways for dif- consequential across the early and middle years of ferent cohorts; and nonnormative influences reflect idiosyn- adulthood. crasies (such as physical) (see Stearns, Chapter 20, this Changes occur in relation to positive and nega- Handbook, this volume). This conceptualization is unduly tive events, gains, and losses, with the likelihood of restrictive in two senses. First, within-cohort variabil- expected losses increasing. Biological resources decline ity largely reflects non-normative influences, which are over the life span, but cultural resources may increase not easily subject to scientific study (Dannefer, 1984). through the cultivation of wisdom and problem solving. As a result, the social basis for within-cohort differ- Life-span development entails selection, optimization, ences becomes a residual category. Second, as Mayer and compensation. These mechanisms seek to maxi- (2004) noted, life-span psychology views historical and mize gains and minimize losses or declines. Selective nonnormative influences as idiographic (i.e., unique, non- optimization with compensation represents a “life-span repeating), leaving only age-graded influences, which are model of psychological nature of human aging and thought to be largely based on biology and age norms. the ubiquitous, age-related shift toward a less positive Because the larger social forces that lead to age norms balance of gains and losses” (Baltes, 1993, p. 590). are of little interest, within-cohort regularities in behavior are explained solely by personal attributes (biology and The way these mechanisms or strategies work in later institutionalized norms). life is illustrated by an interview with the renowned concert In the final analysis, the study of contextual influences in pianist Arthur Rubenstein. When asked how he remained a cohorts is hampered because it produces largely invariant successful pianist in his later years, Rubenstein referred to patterns through such age-graded influences, or it cannot be three strategies: “(1) he performed fewer pieces, (2) he now studied because of its seemingly random nature. Some of practiced each more frequently, and (3) he introduced more these issues were dampened by the initial enthusiasm of ritardandos in his playing between fast segments, so that the Baltes for cohort studies and the analysis of interindividual playing sounded faster than it was” (Baltes, 1993, p. 590). differences in intraindividual change. But in retrospect, The strategy of selection is illustrated by Rubenstein’s con- it appears that Baltes’s volume on cohort studies with centration on fewer pieces, the more frequent practice illus- Nesselroade in 1979 was followed by a decline in his trates the use of optimization, and the increasing reliance on regard for them. Nevertheless, some life-span investigators contrast in speed exemplifies a strategy of compensation. (e.g., Heckhausen, 1999) have continued to assess the link This psychological model of successful aging has rele- between broader social contexts and individual functioning vance for development at all ages including childhood and across the life course. In the field of developmental science, adolescence. Adaptations in adolescence can be viewed there are numerous examples of this line of work, such through the guidelines of selective optimization in which as Silbereisen’s Jena research program on social change gains are maximized and risks, losses, or deprivation and human development, with its featured research on the are minimized (see Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, impact of German Unification. We provide an overview of 2010). Youth select activities in which they are competent this research on pages 44–47. (e.g., athletics, academics, military service, or street life) and optimize benefits through an investment of resources, Social Relations: Roles and Sequences time, energy, and relationships. Life-span developmen- talists such as Baltes have enriched our thinking about The second column of Figure 2.1 refers to how an individ- development and aging across the life course, and they ual’s life pattern is structured by multiple role sequences, have given some attention to the role of social, cultural, their transitions, and “linked lives,” Transitions into and out and historical forces in developmental processes. of social roles across the life span entail both social and However, their perspective on life-span development personal changes in status and identity (Glaser & Strauss, generally fails to apprehend social structure as a consti- 1971). Changes in major social roles, such as from living tutive force in development. The problem stems from the with parents in a dependent role and then moving to an framework’s conceptualization of context—it refers to independent household with a spouse, generally represent age-graded, history-graded, or nonnormative influences. a change in life stage to the status of an adult. This process Age-graded influences shape individual development in involves human agency in the selection of role options as largely normative ways for all people; history-graded well as social influences and constraints. 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 11 The life cycle represented a dominant model of the has become increasingly uncoupled from marriage. Chil- social life span from the early 1900s to the 1960s. In its dren are increasingly born prior to marriage or outside of most precise definition, life cycle refers to a sequence of marriage altogether. In the United States, the prevalence of roles in parenting, from the birth of children through their divorce from the 1960s to the present has led to multiple maturity and departure from the home to the birth of their families in a person’s life and to the likelihood that most own children. In a life cycle of generational succession, children will experience a single-parent household before newborns are socialized to maturity, give birth to the they enter adulthood (e.g., Fussell, 2002). Even with these next generation, grow old, and die. The cycle is repeated limitations, the life-cycle concept and its family cycle tell from one generation to the next in a human population us much about the social matrix of one’s life—the linked (O’Rand & Krecker, 1990). As reproductive cycles, the lives. They knit together a full array of family relationships life cycle can vary greatly in tempo through variations in through life stages and the generations, providing insight the timing of childbirth, whether very early or late in life into family processes such as socialization and social con- between the generations. trol over the life span. They connect the developing person Role change in one generation has consequences across and his or her career. the generations, ascending and descending (Burton, 1985). Another feature of this complexity emerged as mothers When the eldest daughter has a child before the age of 13, increased their involvement in the labor force over the her mother may become a grandmother before the age of 30 last quarter of the 20th century. This upward trend posed and a great-grandmother before the age of 50. A sequence another limitation for the life-cycle framework and sug- of early childbearing across the generations weakens the gested the need for a dual career perspective to study these generational and age foundation for family authority and families and the lives of their members. However, even in social control. Family authority over a newborn child tends the early 1970s, a prime era for life-cycle research, Young to shift upward from the teenage mother to the grand- and Willmott (1973) found that studies of work and family mother. By contrast, late childbearing slows the cycle and were typically proceeding along separate paths with no minimizes age similarities across adjacent generations. substantial effort to investigate their interdependence and Entry into later-life relationships may provide the social coordination problems. This observation contrasts rather control to stabilize a person’s life and minimize involve- strikingly today with a flourishing study of the interlocking ment in unconventional and dangerous activities. In their trajectories of work and family life (Drobnic, Blossfield, Boston sample, Sampson and Laub (1993) reported that & Rohwer, 1999; Moen, 2003). Life course models have bonds to conventional figures provided a route of escape been constructed to capture this dynamic. from delinquency for a number of men with a childhood In all of these ways, the life cycle of family roles history of delinquency. entailed shortcomings in thinking about the life course During the familistic post–World War II years, the life of children and their parents. The temporality of age cycle became well known as the family cycle, through addresses some of these limitations by supplementing the writings of Glick and Hill, as a set of ordered stages its relational approach with a temporal and contextual of parenthood defined primarily by variations in family perspective. Entry into social roles in the life cycle may composition and size (Elder, 1978). Major transition points follow a certain temporal order, but these role transitions included courtship, engagement, marriage, birth of the first are not temporally located in a person’s life. For example, and last child, the children’s transitions in school, departure a life-cycle model of a person’s life might locate marriage of the eldest and youngest child from the home, and marital before the first birth, but it would not indicate whether the dissolution through the death of one spouse. This sequence marriage occurred at 20 or 40 years. The evidence suggests of life stages is based on a concept of marriage that bears that event timing matters because social timetables, age children and remains intact up to old age and death. Deviant norms, and age-graded sanctions influence behavior. patterns are excluded, such as marriages without children, The concept of generation in the life-cycle perspective those preceded by children, the widowed and divorced occupies a common historical location relative to histor- whether with or without children, and serial marriages ical events such as the economic recession that occurred (see also Ganong, Coleman, & Russell, Chapter 4, this between 1980 and 1983. A parent generation may have Handbook, this volume). birth years that span 30 years, a period that could include The emerging complexity of contemporary family life eras of economic boom and bust in the 20th century. did not fit this concept of the life cycle. First, childbearing As such, it is apparent that generational role or position 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 12 Human Development in Time and Place cannot offer a precise way of connecting people’s lives Before Ryder’s essay on cohorts, the birth years of to changes in society, whereas age and/or birth year does study members in surveys and longitudinal studies were offer such a perspective. most unlikely to be considered a way to locate people in A social role-generation perspective and a temporal- history. Even the historical context of empirical studies contextual perspective based on age are complemen- received minimal attention (Thernstrom, 1964), although tary in thinking about the social life course embedded Bronfenbrenner (1958) demonstrated the importance of in a social-historical context. One of the best research doing so by showing that the findings of two surveys examples of why this convergence is important comes from of social class and childrearing made sense when one The Polish Peasant in Europe and America (Thomas & noted that they were carried out in different eras of the Znaniecki, 1918). This pioneering work was described in 20th century. Ryder’s influential essay increased the sen- the 1960s as “the greatest single study done thus far by an sitivity of social scientists to the historical context of lives American sociologist” (Nisbet, 1969, p. 316). The lives and their birth cohorts. of immigrants embodied the discontinuities of the age; In addition, the surge of newly initiated longitudinal they were socialized for a world that had become only studies provided a dynamic approach to age and its mean- a memory. The societies they left and entered—the Old ings across the life span. This fresh perspective on age World and the New—presented contrasting pathways for reflected the pioneering work of Neugarten (Neugarten, individual adaptation and development. Matters of social 1968, 1996; Neugarten & Datan, 1973) at the University of and historical time are clearly relevant to this project, Chicago’s Committee on Human Development during the and yet, Thomas and Znaniecki were largely insensitive late 1950s and 1960s. Her work with colleagues revealed to them. the variability of lives. People do not move across their For many years, the social role/life cycle perspective lives in concert with others of the same age. They vary in continued to offer a valuable way of thinking about the the age at which they enter and leave key social roles. social patterning and interdependence of lives, although In what follows, we more fully describe contributions limited in a number of respects that we have noted. In the to the two research traditions on age and the life course, 1970s, this approach was combined with new understand- the link between age cohorts and an age-graded perspective ings of age to form life course models with the analytic on life patterns. In combination, they bring temporality and virtues of both theoretical traditions: linked lives across context to a social perspective on the life course. the life span and generations, coupled with the temporal- ity of age and context through an age-graded sequence A Cohort-Historical Perspective of events and social roles, embedded in birth cohorts. Birth year or date of entry into a system such as school grad- These models were also enriched by life-span concepts of uation locates the individual according to historical time human development that feature the agency of individuals and related social change. With age peers in the cohort, in constructing their lives. the individual is exposed to a particular segment of histori- cal experience as he or she moves across age-graded roles. To grasp the meaning and implications of birth year and Age and the Life Course cohort membership, the investigator specifies the distinc- A greater understanding of the meanings of age in peo- tive historical events and processes at the time as well as ple’s lives during the 1950s and 1960s provided a way of characteristics of the cohort, such as its size and compo- thinking about the relation of historical location and its sition. These characteristics are themselves a consequence ecology to life patterns with its events and social roles of historical changes in birth and death rates, immigration, across the life span. The link between age/birth year and and migration. historical time occurred in large part through the influential As successive cohorts encounter the same historical essay of Ryder (1965) on the cohort as a way of studying event, they do so at different life stages, defined by social social change and its effects on people and populations. roles, maturity, and life experiences. This means that adja- Riley et al. wrote a comprehensive work on this topic in cent cohorts bring different life experiences to the change. Aging and Society (1972). This important volume relates Ryder (1965, p. 846) stressed this life-stage principle in his birth cohorts and age-graded roles. Both Ryder and Riley account of cohort differences in the life course. As each provided conceptual models for this relatively undeveloped cohort encounters a historical event, whether depression field of study at the time. or prosperity, it “is distinctively marked by the career 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License The Development of Life Course Theory 13 stage it occupies.” This mark may take different forms. characteristics in samples of middle-class adults. The data One type of outcome involves cohort differences, such not only show a general agreement among men and women as the less adverse effects of hardship among the older on the appropriate age for a woman to marry but also Oakland boys in the Great Depression study than among support the hypothesis that informal sanctions are associ- the much younger Berkeley boys (Elder, 1974/1999). ated with relatively early and late marriage. Moreover, the For another perspective, consider age at entry into World women were aware if they were on time, late, or early with War II. The age range spanned 20 years: Some recruits respect to marriage and other major role transitions. were launching their adult lives, whereas others were in This pioneering line of research has been extended in their mid-30s with families and careers. fruitful ways by Settersten. He and Hagestad carried out a In addition to cohort effects, history may take the form study of the perceived deadlines in both family and educa- of a period effect when the influence of a historical change tion/work transitions in the 1990s among men and women is relatively uniform across all age groups. Rodgers and in the Chicago area (Settersten, 2003). A large majority of Thornton (1985) found that marriage and divorce rates did the respondents claimed that there were deadlines for this not vary across the 20th century by age groups. On rates type of transition, but Settersten noted that the big challenge of marital dissolution, they observed that “the big picture is in this area is to clarify what is meant by the term age norm. one of overwhelmingly historical effects that influenced all Research on age norms has been limited by the fact that the subgroups of the population substantially and surprisingly identification of an age norm typically requires the observa- equally” (p. 29). Concerning divorce, they referred espe- tion of a relevant sanction—the two phenomena cannot be cially to the rising level up to the 1930s, the decline in the studied independently. Settersten also made the point that Great Depression era, a rapid recovery to the extraordinary deviations from age expectations and timetables may entail peak of divorce in the mid-1940s, and then to the upward consequences that have nothing to do with informal sanc- trend during the 1960s and 1970s. The precise explanation tions as we know them. A very late marriage, for example, for such period influences was not determined. increases the risk of childlessness. When theory and research focus on the cohort level, the For many decades, age-grades were inferred as possess- linking mechanisms between lives and changing times have ing common significance for members without evidence been difficult to pin down. Cohorts can be merely “black of their meaning to these individuals. When do young boxes” with no information on causal dynamics and link- people assume the perspective of an adult? Neugarten was ages. Speculation frequently takes the place of disciplined one of the first developmentalists to pose such questions, explication. Another issue concerns environmental varia- and she did this work with a sample of adults during the tion within cohorts. Thus, some children may be exposed 1950s. She found (see Neugarten & Peterson, 1957) that to the economic stress of a plant closing, whereas others men with lower socioeconomic status tended to perceive a are insulated from such stresses by their father’s different more rapid passage through the major age divisions of life place of employment. In response to such heterogeneity, than did middle-class men: Maturity, middle age, and old more studies are investigating specific types of differential age came earlier in the lower SES strata, owing perhaps social change within a single birth cohort (George, 2009). to class-linked laboring jobs and stresses. The men who The age-graded life course. During the late 1950s relied on mental skills in a sedentary occupation foresaw a and early 1960s, Neugarten directed a research program relatively long period of productivity, whereas the manual that featured the concept of a normative timetable and worker expected a relatively short span of productive individual deviations from age expectations. The timetable activity, followed by retirement. refers to the social meanings of age, as defined by people’s Contemporary studies of the meanings of age status expectations regarding events and social roles. In theory, have focused on the transition to adulthood. Entry into age expectations specify appropriate times for major family roles (marital and especially parental) are typically transitions, and violations of them may lead to adverse most predictive of an adult definition of self, and this is consequences, from informal sanctions to lost opportu- what Shanahan, Mortimer, and Porfeli (2005) observed nities. There is an appropriate time for entering school, from a contemporary longitudinal study of the young adult leaving home, getting married, having children, and retir- transition in an urban sample of midwestern Americans. ing from the labor force. With colleagues (Neugarten, It is also the case that entry into these roles has been Moore, & Lowe, 1965), Neugarten observed a high degree delayed significantly across the 20th century, owing in of consensus on age norms across some 15 age-related part to employment and advanced education opportunities. 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 14 Human Development in Time and Place Consistent with this interpretation, Americans in their paradigm is responsive to the call by Lerner (1991, p. 27) 20s who perceived themselves to be relatively late in the for more attention to contextual variability and represents transition to adulthood were found to be most committed a perspective in the field of developmental science (Cairns, to an advanced path of higher education in a national Elder, & Costello, 1996) that extends across system levels longitudinal study (Benson & Elder, 2011). Young peo- and disciplines. ple who defined themselves as adults ranked lowest in The contextual perspective of the life course framework socioeconomic origin and educational plans. Similar to has much in common with Bronfenbrenner’s ecology Neugarten and Peterson’s finding of life course acceler- of human development, now called bioecology theory ation in later life among adults in the lower SES, these (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994). His Ecology of Human young people were following an accelerated subjective Development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) proposed a multi- path to adult status. level view of the sociocultural environment, from macro Research on age and the subjective life course represents to micro, but it did not include a temporal perspective on an example of how investigations of the meanings of age individual development across changing environments. have opened up a way to think about identity in a context Some years later, Bronfenbrenner (1989, p. 201) noted of changing roles across the life course. The sequence of this major lacuna in his work and proposed the concept of age-graded roles and statuses depicts a social trajectory chronosystem with its three interacting components over of the life course, and its transitions from one role to time: (1) the developing person; (2) the changing envi- another that influence how young people view themselves ronment; and (3) their proximal processes. This concept and others. has not been widely adopted, but Bronfenbrenner’s eco- logical perspective has appeared in numerous contextual studies of child development, especially in the field of Converging Research Traditions in Life Course Theory neighborhood influences (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Contemporary theory on the life course and its social Leventhal, Dupéré, & Shuey, Chapter 13, this Handbook, dimensions differs from the perspectives of an earlier this volume). era by joining the life-cycle processes of social relation- Human development in life course theory represents a ships with the temporal and contextual aspects of age. process of organism-environment transactions over time In Children of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999), in which the organism plays an active role in shaping its the social role perspective was combined with the analytic own development. The developing person is viewed as a meanings of age for linking family and individual experi- dynamic whole, not as separate strands, facets, or domains ence to historical change, and for identifying age-graded such as emotion, cognition, and motivation. The course of trajectories across the life course. Both theoretical strands development is embedded in a dynamic system of social provide essential features of a social life course on matters interchanges and interdependencies across and within of time, context, and process. The life course is age-graded levels. As noted by Bronfenbrenner (1996), this dynamic through social institutions and structures, and embedded in in life course theory is illustrated well by the interlocking relationships that constrain and support behavior. In addi- lives and developmental trajectories of family members tion, people are located in historical settings through birth who are influenced differently by their changing world. cohorts and are linked across the generations by ties of We turn now to perspectives and basic concepts that link kinship and friendship. the social life course and developmental processes. By integrating social relationship concepts and age- based distinctions on social trajectories, along with life- span concepts of the person, the life course framework ELEMENTARY LIFE COURSE CONCEPTS offered a promising approach to the contextual study of AND PERSPECTIVES human development in longitudinal samples (Figure 2.1). Both the individual life course and a person’s developmen- For a study that is framed in terms of changing times tal trajectory are connected with the lives and development and places, the objective is to link historical and spatial of others. Life course theory thus took issue with life-span processes with individual development by examining studies that viewed human development as an unfold- multiple levels of the social environment (Elder & Russell, ing process that is not coactive with social and cultural 2000). Starting with the macro level, societal change may processes in historical time. Moreover, the life course transform social institutions, communities, and cultures, 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License Elementary Life Course Concepts and Perspectives 15 and in so doing establish developmental constraints for TABLE 2.1 Central Concepts of the Life Course: Social and Developmental Dynamics choices and generate individual agency at the micro level. The multilevel nature of the life course and human devel- Conceptual Description Examples opment invites different points of entry, each with specific Social Pathway: Sequences of Tracking within schools; questions, ranging from cultures and social institutions positions within and between occupational career ladders; institutions, organizations, and transitions from school tracks to to human biology and the genome (Shanahan & Porfeli, phases of life labor markets 2002). A single study commonly employs different entry Duration: Time spent in a social Years in poverty; years married points for aspects of the same project. Thus a project moti- status or role, span of exposure vated by the impact of rural change on children’s social Cumulative Effects: Increasing Effect of education on health and emotional development should be framed by an initial effect of earlier experiences with becomes stronger as people age the passage of time (akin to focus on the transformation of rural communities and the compounding interest) economic well-being and hardships of families within Chains of Interrelated Events: Chain of risk: Life events often these communities. Such a study would be incomplete Sequences of risky or salutary lead to further life-events; without reference to the adaptive patterns of parents and experiences across development institutionalization in childhood children: their developmental trajectories of behaviors, increases likelihood of additional risks psychological profiles, and health. Social Trajectory: Behaviors that Income stream from an Indeed, empirical studies of the farm crisis (1980 to likely coincide with pathways occupational career line early 1990s) in the United States, as it played out in central Developmental Trajectory: Pattern of change in depressed Iowa, tell us that the distinction between families engaged Behavioral pattern over time, often affect through adolescence is in farming versus families living in the small rural towns associated with coinciding social associated with patterns of patterns in context stressors during the same period was key to linking social change and young people’s lives. Transitions: Discrete change in Transition to first grade, to Parts of this study might also investigate the determinants social role, set of roles, or adulthood, to a new school of specific emotional or social outcomes and relevant pro- membership in social organization tective resources in the family, a point of entry that centers Turning Point: Change in social Transition to a new school may be on the developmental status of the child. Still other entry circumstances that markedly alters associated with substantial life course, often because of the improvement points might begin with the interchange of parents and child meaning of the event or with sibling relationships. Each point could become a Knifing-Off Experience: Turning Military service can interrupt framing statement for an independent study, although all point that renders earlier life course nascent antisocial career; marriage entry points provide insight into a central guiding question much less consequential may have similar effect about context. By studying diverse aspects of the same problem, the processes of social change and individual on the properties of social transitions (see Table 2.1). development give life to variables otherwise considered Third, we focus on linking mechanisms that have proven “social addresses,” such as SES, sex, and ethnicity. highly useful in the study of change and place. Beginning Considerable leverage in conducting such studies is with studies of children who were born before the Great provided by concepts and perspectives that bridge social Depression, research has revealed a set of mechanisms that change, place, and individual development, theoretical link context and the individual life course and, as will be tools that reflect decades of empirical study. To understand seen in subsequent sections, these mechanisms have proven this conceptual bridge, we turn to elementary concepts. highly probative in the study of place. The paradigmatic First, we begin with multiple levels of the life course, themes of life course theory draw on these elementary con- ranging from institutionalized pathways to cumulative cepts and mechanisms, underscoring the socially dynamic patterns of context, which shape the individual life course. basis for individual development. Developmental science is ultimately directed to the study of adaptive and maladaptive patterns of change and Social Pathways, Cumulative Processes, and Durations constancy at the level of the person. Yet institutionalized pathways provide a broad context for development and set Social pathways, cumulative patterns, and the duration of the stage for cumulative patterns of social experiences that experiences represent dynamic views of context. Pathways shape the individual’s life course. Second, other temporally typically refer to sequences of social positions in and sensitive concepts—most notably, trajectory, transition, between organizations, institutions, and phases of life. and turning point—are taken up with particular emphasis Institutionalized pathways generally have specified time 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy402 by Columbia University Libraries, Wiley Online Library on [08/01/2025]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License 16 Human Development in Time and Place boundaries, what Merton (1984) called “socially expected particular cohorts, and state intervention. Growth of the durations.” Children who are held back in school become state in social regulation counters the modern tendency aware of their lagging status on the educational ladder toward individualism. At the personal level, the state (Alexander, Entwisle, & Dauber, 1994), and company “legalizes, defines and standardizes most points of entry managers talk about the relation between age and grade and exit: into and out of employment, into and out of in prospects for promotion to senior rank (Sofer, 1970, marital status, into and out of sickness and disability, into p. 239). A growing body of research also considers early and out of education. In doing so, the state turns these entry into adult roles—what Burton (2007) aptly called transitions into strongly demarcated public events and acts “adultification”—as well as pathways into retirement (Kim as gatekeeper and sorter” (p. 167). To be sure, each nation- & Moen, 2002). Whether a new phase of life, emerging state represents a unique configuration of laws, rules, adulthood, now characterizes pathways into adulthood is a and norms that structure the life course. Viewed from lively topic of inquiry (Bynner, 2005). this vantage point, cross-national and historical studies In addition to their age-graded nature, pathways struc- become highly strategic in studying societal forces and ture the direction that people’s lives can take. Pallas (2003, individual lives and indeed they have become increasingly p. 168–169) observed that pathways have distinct fea- common as diverse countries collect data containing the tures that govern how strongly people’s trajectories and same information. The Panel Study of Income Dynamics in behaviors are shaped, including, for example, the number the United States (launched in 1968) has become a model of options a pathway leaves open in the future, the extent for nationwide longitudinal studies in Europe, as in Great of mobility that is likely to be experienced, stigma and Britain and Germany. extrinsic rewards, and the importance of personal choice. Multilevel accounts of the life course are well illus- Some pathways provide future opportunities and chances trated by studies of the transition to adulthood (Settersten, for upward mobility based on personal motivation, whereas Furstenberg, & Rumbaut, 2005), which highlight how others effectively block promising avenues irrespective of changing institutional arrangements and cultural under- one’s efforts. Importantly, these pathways reflect social standings shape pathways by comparing and contrasting arrangements as found, for example, by McFarland (2006) different countries and historical periods. Billari noted that in how a particular high school chooses to implement a such comparisons are especially powerful among European math curriculum. countries. Each has distinct socioeconomic, political, and Pathways are also multilevel phenomena reflecting cultural features and yet, particularly with the formation of arrangements in place at levels of culture, the nation-state, the European Union, they have a growing sense of com- social institutions and organizations, and locale. To varying mon identity (Billari & Liefbroer, 2010). His empirical degrees, people work out their life course in established or work suggests that the transition to adulthood is becom- institutionalized pathways. At the macro end of