Coping Styles in Adolescents Facing Terrorism (2009) PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by SuitableSanDiego
Katholieke Universiteit Leuven
2009
Orna Braun-Lewensohn
Tags
Summary
This research investigates coping strategies among Israeli adolescents aged 12-18 facing ongoing terrorism. The study found that adolescents primarily employ problem-solving coping mechanisms, while emotional focused coping and non-productive coping are associated with negative psychological outcomes. Exposure, appraisal (perceived danger), and coping styles explain a large proportion of the variance in adolescents' psychological well-being.
Full Transcript
Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 www.elsevier.com/locate/jado Coping styles as moderating the relationships between terrorist attacks and well-being outcomes Orna Br...
Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 www.elsevier.com/locate/jado Coping styles as moderating the relationships between terrorist attacks and well-being outcomes Orna Braun-Lewensohn a,b,*, Smadar Celestin-Westreich b, Leon-Patrice Celestin c, Gino Verleye d, Dominique Verté e, Ingrid Ponjaert-Kristoffersen b a Department of General Studies, Conflict Management Program, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel b Department of Developmental & Lifespan Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Belgium c Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hospital Poissy-Saint-Germain-en-Laye, France d Department of Communication Sciences, University Gent, Gent, Belgium e Department of Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Belgium Abstract This study aims to explore use of coping strategies among adolescents and their relationships with well being in the context of ongoing terrorism. Furthermore, we aim to explore to what extent coping styles in addition to exposure variables explain well being of adolescents facing ongoing terror. During September 2003, after three years of ongoing terror attacks, 913 Israeli adolescents aged 12e18 years, completed the following questionnaires during regular class sessions: Demographics, Achenbach’s Youth Self Report; Exposure to Terror and Post Traumatic Stress (PTS) questionnaire; Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS) and Brief Symptoms Inventory. Adolescents employed mainly problem solving strategies which mean they have the capacity to cope well in spite of stressful events. Emotional focused coping was associated with PTS and mental health problems. Regression analysis of different exposure and coping variables revealed that exposure, appraisal (subjective exposure) and coping styles explained 26e37% of the variance of different psychological problems. The findings highlight the importance of appraisal (subjective exposure) and coping strategies, for under- standing adolescents’ mental health outcomes. Moreover, these findings are relevant to the development of * Corresponding author. Department of General Studies, Conflict Management Program, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer Sheva, Israel. Tel.: þ972 54 5382524; fax: þ972 50 8971146. E-mail address: [email protected] (O. Braun-Lewensohn). 0140-1971/$30.00Ó2008TheAssociationforProfessionalsinServicesforAdolescents.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.Allrightsreserved. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.003 586 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 prevention/intervention programs that facilitate youth’s cognitive and emotional adjustments to ongoing trauma risks and terror threats. Ó 2008 The Association for Professionals in Services for Adolescents. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Adolescents; Terrorism; Coping; Well-being; PTSS/D; Psychological problems Introduction The present study aims to explore the use of coping strategies by adolescents facing ongoing terrorism. It also aims to strengthen insight into the process of adjustment cognitively and emotionally to ongoing terror risks by investigating relationships between coping styles and mental health outcomes. The importance of coping has indeed been stressed throughout research in recent years. Generally, coping strategies have been found to moderate the relationship between exposure to stress and anxiety outcomes. Coping can be defined as the actual effort that is made in the attempt to render a perceived stressor more tolerable and minimize the distress induced by the situation. Most models of coping assume that individuals who cope more effectively with stressful life events show lower levels of anxiety or depression (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Thus, different studies in the domains of adolescents’ coping with stressful situation show that emotion focused strategies of reference to others and non-productive coping tend to be associated with more psychological problems while problem focused strategies or active coping tend to be linked to more well being (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1999; Lewis & Frydenberg, 2002). The period of adolescence is crucial to the development of coping skills. Adolescents more readily tend to engage in experiences or encounter situations that are associated with increased risks for developing emotional and behavioral problems. Teenagers are also at the stage of developing their personal styles of coping. It is during these years that from one experience of using certain mechanisms of coping to another, coping strategies can be reviewed, modified as needed and crystallized (Frydenberg, 1997). From puberty on, youth also develop more advanced cognitive and emotional mastery, enabling them to see the perspective of others, plan ahead to see future consequences of an action and manage emotions more effectively thus, facilitating their abilities to deal with sources of conflict, and threatening or stressful events in a variety of contexts (Garnefski, Legerstee, Kraaij, Van den Kommer, & Teerds, 2002; Xianchen, Jenn-Yun, & Zhongtang, 2004). Research in different domains of exposure to potentially traumatic experiences has shown that similar exposure does not necessarily lead to similar extents of psychological problems among individuals. Research repeatedly demonstrates that dynamic processes such as coping tend to function as strong mediators between stressors and their mental health outcomes, notably in the context of various types of emotional and behavioral problems in youth (Aldwin, 1994; Celestin & Celestin-Westreich, 2006; Dempsey, 2002). When attempting to understand the psychological implications of adolescents’ exposure to terrorism, recent studies converge to suggest similar relevance of a coping perspective for our insight into the dynamics through which O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 587 experiences of terror attacks impact on youth’s well being (Cardena, Dennis, Winkel, & Skitka, 2005; Gil & Caspi, 2006; Zeidner, 2005). Despite this evidence and although research of adolescents exposure to terrorist acts has grown consistently in recent years, few studies to date have specifically addressed the usage of coping mechanisms in adolescent populations in the context of facing terrorism (e.g. Cardena et al., 2005; Wadsworth et al., 2004). In the Israeli context of exposure to ongoing terrorism, Zeidner (2005) explored the use of coping strategies of adolescents in Haifa confronted with a continuing threat of terrorism. Another study in the context of the PalestinianeIsraeli conflict explored trait coping (pre-attack) and state coping (post attack) of first year undergraduate students before and after a bus explosion in Haifa (Gil & Caspi, 2006). These studies found emotion focused strategies and avoidance coping to be meaningful predictors of maladaptive outcomes while problem focused strategies were found to be a weak predictor or did not predict stress or other psycholog- ical problems at all. Since research in the broader context of war, including terrorism, leads to conflicting results as to which coping strategies come with more or less psychological distress, some findings between suggestive of avoidance and denial as effective strategies while others advocate the importance of problem focused coping (Muldoon & Cairns, 1999; Zeidner, 1993, 2005). Further exploration of the role of coping styles in the context of ongoing terrorism is needed. Exposure to terror attacks When investigating adolescents’ mental health outcomes in the context of terrorism, most research to date has quite naturally considered direct, physical exposure to the attacks as being a primary factor in determining subsequent PTSD, emotional and behavioral problems. Several studies have demonstrated that higher levels of physical exposure to terrorist attacks elicit more adverse psychological reactions, such as higher rates of PTSD, anxiety and func- tional impairment (e.g. Hoven et al., 2002). However, after terrorist attacks this type of expo- sure appears to explain a limited amount of variance in youth’s mental health problems (Pfefferbaum et al., 2001). An additional type of exposure via relationship with a victim has been postulated as a significant moderating variable especially when focusing on children or adolescents, given that youth’s developmental status may render them more vulnerable to the loss of loved-ones (Pynoss & Eth, 1985). Several studies showed that adolescents who knew a victim experienced more PTSD and stress related symptoms than those who did not know a victim (e.g. Hoven et al., 2002; Pfefferbaum et al., 1999; Ronen, Rahav, & Appel, 2003). In the context of terrorism, subjective exposure was studied less. However, reports of initial fears and worries for safety of family members and friends after the Oklahoma City and Nairobi bombings seemed to be significant predictors of PTSD (Pfefferbaum et al., 2002; Pfefferbaum et al., 2003). In addition, when subjective exposure was defined by fear levels, adolescents who were more exposed physically were not always found to report higher sense of fear (Solomon, Laufer, & Lavi, 2005). Pat Horenczyk (2003) addressed another concept of exposure; ‘near miss’ experience (e.g. ‘miss the bus that later exploded’). She found that youths who were less physically exposed and knew fewer people who were hurt in terrorist attacks, reported more PTSD symptoms, overall distress 588 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 and malfunctioning in the domains of family functions and risk-taking behavior. A suggested explanation for these results maybe that those adolescents reported more near miss experiences. Media exposure as an additional dimension of exposure seems to contribute to post traumatic stress symptoms even for those who weren’t directly exposed (Pfefferbaum et al., 2001). A longi- tudinal study estimating the effects of the September 11th attacks on adolescents’ mental health concluded that while neither physical nor family exposure predicted change in mental health after September 11th, media exposure did predict an increase in PTSD symptoms (Aber, Gershoff, Ware, & Kotler, 2004). These examples suggest that the media have significant role in determining mental health outcomes following terror attacks. Thus, this study aims to examine how adolescents are affected by situational determinants in different types and levels of exposure to terrorist attacks in a broader framework taking into consideration also primary appraisal as subjective exposure (perception of endangerment) and coping styles. Emotional reactions to terror attacks Recent research of adolescents’ responses to terrorist attacks and prolonged exposure to political violence has shown these incidents to have a substantial impact on their emotional and behavioral functioning. Adverse mental health outcomes are indeed found to vary widely both in terms of type and extent, ranging from mild stress reactions, through PTSD to psychopathological responses including somatic complaints, depression, anxiety, conduct disorder, functional impairment, panic attacks etc. (e.g. Hoven et al., 2002; Pat Horenczyk & Doppelt, 2005; Solomon & Lavi, 2005; Thabet & Vostanis 2002). Studies to date have mainly identified factors such as age, gender, prior trauma and type of exposure to diversely mediate these posttraumatic and mental health difficulties. However, these variables appear to explain only relatively limited amounts of the variance in mental health outcomes. For example, gender was found to explain about 2e12% while different types of exposure explain 3e13% in youth’s terror-related mental health difficulties across studies (Pfeffrbaum et al., 2002; Pfefferbaum et al., 2001; Solomon et al., 2005). Therefore, it seems that the role of coping styles may shed some light into the process in which adolescents facing ongoing terror do or do not develop psychological problems. Research background and aims During the al-Aqsa Intifada which started in late September 2000, Israeli society has been con- fronted with a wave of terrorist attacks, including, among others, drive-by shootings, break-ins and suicide bombings. By September 2003, when the present study was conducted, numerous children and adolescents had witnessed such attacks directly or indirectly, thus raising questions about the psychological impact of these potentially traumatic experiences. The aim of this study was to explore the use of coping strategies among adolescents who faced ongoing threat of terrorist attacks over several years. We investigated the relationships between the different coping strategies and exposure variables as well as the relationships between the coping strategies and the different psychological and behavioral problems. Finally, we investi- gated the role of exposure variables and coping styles to explain the well-being of adolescents following terror attacks. We formulated the following research hypotheses: O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 589 1 Based on previous research we hypothesize that adolescents throughout Israel will mainly use problem solving strategies to deal with the stressful situation of ongoing terrorism (Zeidner, 2005). 2 Problem solving strategies will be linked to better well being while non-productive coping or coping with reference to others will be linked to more psychological problems (Cardena et al., 2005; Zeidner, 2005). 3 The different exposure variables and the different coping scales will have contributed to explain- ing the different psychological problems. Thus, subjective exposure as well as ‘non-productive’ coping are expected to be the most significant contributors to the different psychological problems (Gil & Caspi, 2006; Solomon et al., 2005; Zeidner, 2005). While non-productive coping is expected to have a negative effect, problem solving is expected to have positive contribution (Dempsey, 2002). Methods Population sample This study has comprised a sample of 913 Israeli adolescents aged 12e18 years. No other inclusion or exclusion criteria were used aside from age and availability at the time the questionnaires were administrated. The demographic characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1. Students attending four different schools (junior and senior high schools) in four locations in Israel were chosen to represent different levels of exposure. Three of the schools (Central Israel, Southern Israel and Jordan Valley) belong to the same educational sub-system (Department of Rural Educa- tion), which is part of the ‘Mamlachty’ educational system and the only one that has schools scattered throughout Israel. Most of the schools under this system are located in suburbs or in rural areas, with students living in kibbutzim1, moshavim2 or small towns. The Jerusalem school, in turn, serves different neighborhoods of Jerusalem and nearby small towns. All participating schools are ‘‘open access’’ (no selective admission procedures). In the Jordan Valley School, the entire age-relevant pop- ulation participated. For the other schools, three classes from each grade (out of 6e9 classes), with an available schedule at the time of research participated. As shown in Table 1, participants were distrib- uted quite evenly regarding gender and age across schools/locations. The sample’s Socio-Economic Status (SES) was obtained via parental work, grouped according to the ‘Central Bureau of Statistics’ classification into three levels (‘low’, ‘average’ and ‘high’, for respectively below average, average and above average wages) (Table 1). Measures Adolescents filled out a comprehensive standardized self-report battery comprising a demo- graphic; ‘Exposure to Terror’ and ‘Post Traumatic Stress’’ (EPTS) Questionnaire designed specifically for this study’s purpose; Achenbach Youth Self Report (YSR, Hebrew version); the 1 Kibbutz: collective farm or settlement in Israel. 2 Moshav: a cooperative settlement of small individual farms in Israel. 590 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 Table 1 Demographic characteristics of sample. Characteristic No. % Age of participants, mean (SD) [range years] 14.45 (1.27) [12e18] Gender Girls 462 50.6 Boys 447 49 Grade level 8th grade 335 36.7 9th grade 302 33.1 11th grade 276 30.2 Age group Youngest 12e13.5 305 33.4 Middle 14e15.5 349 38.2 Oldest 16e18 255 27.9 Schools Central Israel 250 27.4 Jordan Valley 183 20 Southern Israel 226 24.8 Jerusalem 254 27.8 Socio-economic status Low 242 26.1 Average 487 52.5 High 116 12.5 ‘Adolescent Coping Scale’ (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993a); Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) of Derogatis. Demographics The demographic inventory included questions regarding age, class, school, gender, place of residence and parental work. Exposure to Terror and Post Traumatic Stress Questionnaire (EPTS) We designed this questionnaire to comprehensively assess subjects’ exposure from a multidi- mensional perspective (part 1) along with self-reported Post Traumatic Stress symptoms (part 2). The items regarding the different types of exposure were subsequently combined into indexes as follows: The ‘‘Global Objective Exposure Index’’ included items regarding the number of attacks one was exposed to, geographical location at the time of each the attack, time of the attacks in relation to the investigation as well as items regarding the closeness of relationship to a terror-attack victim, the number of victims known, severity and time of injury. Some items were presented in tables so youngsters had to mark each relevant cell. For example, one table included the type of a terror attack in addition to the time (e.g. in the past week, in the past month etc.). In a different table the student had to mark the geographical location at the time of the attack O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 591 (e.g. I was at the scene among the people who were hurt.. I wasn’t at the scene but heard the police sirens and the ambulances on their way to the hospital). Other items were presented as simple questions. For example, if an individual answered yes to the question ‘were any of the people you know were hurt?’, he/she had to continue and circle each person they know who was hurt in addition to the severity of injury. Relationships were presented to them from mom/dad to neighbor and others. The ‘‘Subjective Exposure Index’’ consists of three items regarding feelings of danger for self, family and friends, along with an item regarding the perceived possibility of losing a family member. Factor analysis was computed yielding one factor with 74.09% of the variance explained. Cronbach alpha reliability for the four items was 0.88. Furthermore, ‘‘Near Miss Experiences’’ included two items regarding ‘‘nearly having been exposed to an attack’’, being ‘‘Did you have to be in a place where a blast occurred and for some reason did not get there (e.g. missed a bus that later exploded)?’’ and ‘‘Have you ever left a place and shortly after a bomb blasted there?’’ Finally, ‘‘Media Exposure’’ consists of one item (After a terror attack do you watch TV, listen to the radio, read newspapers with regards to the attack?) on a 5 point scale, ranging from never to very frequent. The ‘‘Post Traumatic Stress Scale’’ consists of rewording the DSM IV criteria for PTSD into a yes/no question format, thus including 16 items with satisfactory cronbach alpha reliability (alpha ¼ 0.74). Prior to the actual data collection, the EPTS questionnaire was administered to a small panel of youth not belonging to the sample (n ¼ 10), to assure that the items were clear and could easily be understood. Achenbach Youth Self Report The Achenbach Youth Self Report for ages 11e18 (Hebrew version) (Achenbach & Rescola, 2001) measures a broad range of behavioral and emotional problems through 112 items yielding a total problem score along with two broad band scales- (internalizing and externalizing), eight subscales and six DSM scales. The YSR form has proven good internal consistency, testeretest reliability (0.87) and content validity (Achenbach & Rescola, 2001). Cronabach alpha for the Total scale in our study was 0.91. Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS) (Frydenberg & Lewis, 1993b) short form comprises 18 items on a 5 point scale, drawn from the 79 items which compose the original long version. Each of the 18 items represent a coping strategy, which are reduced to three global coping styles labeled ‘‘Problem Focused Coping’’, ‘‘Coping by Reference to Others’’ and ‘‘Non-Productive Coping’’. Given that these are a key component of this research and for the sake of comparability across studies, which often adopt different labels for similar coping strategies/styles, Table 2 provides a full listing of the items constituting each style. According to the manual the three scales have sufficient internal consistency to justify the separate use of these scales. Factor analysis was preformed and three factors appear to correlate those in the long form. Additionally, the three scales appear to discriminate quite satisfactorily and show moderate reliability as well as high correlations with the three global scales from the long version. Cronbach alpha reliability for the global coping scales in our sample was: problem solving ¼ 0.80; reference to others ¼ 0.56 and non-productive coping ¼ 0.70. 592 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 Table 2 Items constituting the three global coping styles. Problem focused Reference to others Non-productive coping Work at solving the problem to the best Talk to other people about Worry about what will happen to me; of my ability; Work hard; Improve my my concern to help me sort Spend more time with boy/girl friend; relationship with others; Look on the it out; Join with people who Improve my relationship with others; Wish bright side of things and think of all have the same concern; Pray a miracle will happen; I have no way of that is good; Make time for leisure for help and guidance so dealing with the situation; Find a way to let activities; Keep fit and healthy that everything will be all off stem- e.g. cry, scream, drink, take drugs right; Ask a professional etc.; Shut myself off from the problem so person for help that I can avoid it; See myself at fault; Don’t let others know how I am feeling Brief Symptom Inventory The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) (Derogatis, 1993) is a 53 item scale on 5 Likert points that measures nine dimensions of psychological and psychiatric problems, summarized in a ‘‘Global Severity Index’’ (GSI). The BSI has good internal consistency for the GSI (0.90) and its subscales (0.68e0.91) (Derogatis, 1993). Due to restrictions made by the Ministry of Education in Israel, five items from this questionnaire had to be omitted (namely, ‘‘Thoughts of ending your life’’, ‘‘Spells of terror or panic’’, ‘‘Feelings that you are watched or talked about by others’’, ‘‘The idea that someone else can control your thoughts’’ and ‘‘The idea that you should be punished for your sins’’). Given that the manual states that omitting up to 25% (13 for the GSI and 1 for the subscales) does not harm the reliability of the scales, only the subscale of ‘‘Psychotics’’ needed to be omitted due to too few remaining items. Cronbach alpha reliability for this sample for the GSI was 0.93. The ACS and BSI questionnaires were translated into Hebrew and back into English to assure accuracy. Procedures In conducting this study, we adhered to all ethical procedures required by the Israeli Ministry of Education. The proposal and questionnaires were sent to the office of the ‘‘Central Scientist’’ and were reviewed both by the ‘Central Scientist’ and the ‘Counseling and Psychological Services’ of the Ministry of Education. After receiving their approval, permission to enter schools was received by each principal following and meeting all requirements made by the Ministry of Education. Data were gathered during the month of September 2003. The self-report questionnaires were filled out anonymously during regular class periods. The time of completion varied according to age, ranging from approximately 25e45 min. Students were informed that the researcher was interested in their experiences as a result of the ongoing terrorist attacks. The researcher informed the students that participation was voluntary, and that should they feel uneasy or uncomfortable at any time during questionnaire administration, they were free to terminate their participation. Few (20 students or less than 2%) chose not to participate and stopped before completing the questionnaire. Many participants expressed enthusiasm while completing the questionnaires, mentioning that they were glad to have the opportunity to share their experiences. O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 593 Data analysis First, frequencies and percentages of the sample’s demographic characteristics were explored. Second, use of global coping styles and specific coping strategies were explored. Third, Pearson correlations were calculated to find out relationships between the coping styles and exposure variables and psychological and behavioral problems. Finally, stepwise regression calculated the explanation of PTS, GSI (BSI) and Total problems of Achenbach by the different exposure variables and coping styles. Statistical analyses were conducted with the statistical software SPSS Version 12 accepted p levels for interpretation being set at alpha < 0.05. Results Adolescents reported using a wide variety of coping strategies to deal with the threat of ongoing terrorist attacks (Table 3). The three items endorsed most frequently to deal with this stressful situ- ation are: ‘‘Make time for leisure activities’’, ‘‘Spend more time with boy/girl friend’’ and ‘‘Look on the bright side of things and think of all that is good’’. Regarding global coping styles, ‘‘Problem Focused Coping’’ comes forward as being most used and ‘‘Reference to Others’’ least in use. The relationships between coping styles, exposure variables and outcome scales are presented in Table 4. Results show that ‘reference to others’ and ‘non-productive’ coping are both linked weakly to all exposure variables except ‘subjective exposure’ which is linked moderately to these Table 3 Styles of coping e range, mean and SD. Coping strategy/scale Range M (SD) Solving the problem 18e90 53.31 (18.11) Reference to others 20e100 37.38 (14.66) Non-productive coping 18e76 40.91 (12.36) Talk to others 1e5 2.29 (1.19) Work at solving the problem to best ability 1e5 2.83 (1.42) Work hard 1e5 2.34 (1.29) Worry 1e5 2.36 (1.28) Spend more time with a friend 1e5 3.26 (1.43) Improve relationships 1e5 2.98 (1.37) Wish for a miracle 1e5 2.71 (1.58) No way of dealing with the situation 1e5 1.89 (1.16) Let off steam 1e5 1.55 (1.07) Join with people with same concern 1e5 1.72 (0.99) Shut off self from the problem 1e5 1.97 (1.21) Being at fault 1e5 1.37 (0.80) Don’t let others know of feelings 1e5 2.38 (1.35) Pray for help and guidance 1e5 2.03 (1.32) Look on the bright side 1e5 3.19 (1.44) Ask for professional help 1e5 1.44 (0.92) Make time for leisure activities 1e5 3.40 (1.49) Keep fit and healthy 1e5 3.04 (1.57) 594 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 Table 4 Relationships between the different coping scales, exposure variables and outcome scales. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. Problem solving e 2. Reference to others 0.47** e 3. Non-productive coping 0.57** 0.57** e 4. Objective exposure 0.03 0.08* 0.09* e 5. Subjective exposure 0.02 0.26** 0.30** 0.18** e 6. Near miss experience 0.06 0.11** 0.13** 0.27** 0.13** e 7. Media exposure 0.10* 0.20** 0.14** 0.14** 0.23** 0.06 e 8. Post traumatic stress 0.07* 0.35** 0.46** 0.23** 0.44** 0.13** 0.14** e 9. Global severity index 0.07* 0.35** 0.49** 0.13** 0.35** 0.13** 0.04 0.69** e 10. Total problems 0.03 0.22** 0.38** 0.13** 0.26** 0.09** 0.05 0.60** 0.69** e coping styles. ‘Solving the problems’ in turn is associated only with media exposure. This associ- ation is very weak. Stronger associations appear between the different coping styles and mental health outcomes compared to the associations with the exposure variables. Thus, ‘non-productive coping’ has the strongest link with each of the global outcome scales. While ‘reference to others’ follow the same pattern but with moderate association, ‘problem solving’ is associated weakly with PTS symptoms and with GSI only. All outcome scales appeared to be significantly related to all exposure scales except media. The strongest correlation appeared between subjective exposure and the different psychological problems. Stepwise regression was computed for the exposure variables and the global coping styles to find out their contribution in explaining the outcome psychological scales (Total, GSI and PTS). A pattern of ‘non-productive coping’ and ‘subjective exposure’ as the dominant variables explaining the different psychological problems appeared. For total Achenbach and GSI scales non-productive coping is the strongest explanatory factor (10.1%; 14.5%) while for PTS it seems that subjective exposure is the strongest contributor with 18.3%. The results mean that more reports of subjective exposure and use of non-productive coping are linked to more psycholog- ical problems (Table 5). As for the other coping styles, problem solving comes forward as an important explanatory factor for the various psychological problems e Total (6.3%); GSI (4.8%) and PTS (3.2%). Usage of more ‘problem solving’ seems to be linked to better well being. ‘Reference to others’ in turn, seems to be the weakest explanatory factor with only 1% of the variance. The exposure variables apart from ‘subjective exposure’ explained limited amount of the variance (1e2%) of the different psychological problems. While ‘objective’ and near miss experi- ences were positively linked to the different psychological problems, media exposure was related negatively to ‘total’ Achenbach scale. Discussion The present study has aimed to investigate adolescents’ use of coping styles when facing ongoing terror attacks. We have further aimed to explore relationships between these styles O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 Table 5 Stepwise regression e exposure and coping variables on global outcomes. Total GSI PTS B b SE t B b SE t B b SE t Step 1 Subjective exposure 2.26 0.09 0.89 2.54** Subjective exposure 0.11 0.14 0.03 4.03*** Subjective exposure 0.90 0.23 0.13 7.10*** Near miss 2.93 0.10 1.03 2.85** Near miss 0.08 0.09 0.03 2.81** Objective exposure 0.17 0.12 0.05 3.74*** Media exposure 2.44 0.13 0.61 4.00*** Near miss 0.29 0.06 0.15 1.99* Objective exposure 0.68 0.07 0.32 2.10** Step 2 Non-productive 0.83 0.53 0.06 12.69*** Non-productive 0.03 0.52 0.00 12.02*** Non-productive 0.11 0.44 0.01 10.71*** Problem solving 0.33 0.31 0.04 7.87*** Problem solving 0.01 0.30 0.00 7.68*** Problem solving 0.04 0.26 0.01 6.76*** Reference to others 0.01 0.13 0.00 3.19** Reference to others 0.03 0.13 0.01 3.50*** R2 ¼ 0.26 R2 ¼ 0.33 R2 ¼ 0.37 *p 0.05 **p 0.01 ***p 0.001. Step 1 e exposure variables; Step 2 e coping strategies. 595 596 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 and different psychological difficulties. Finally, we wanted to discover which factors of exposure and coping explain different emotional and behavioral problems. We hypothesized that Israeli adolescents who face ongoing terrorism and risk of terrorism will tend to use a mostly problem solving style of coping to deal with these stressful events. Our investigation indeed shows that Israeli adolescents, aged 12e18 years, use a wide diversity of coping strategies to deal with the threat of ongoing terrorism with problem solving being most frequently used and reference to others the least style in use. As problem solving strategies seem to reflect a capacity of coping well with stressful events (Lewis & Frydenberg, 2002) it seems that Israeli adolescents even under such continuing stressful event illustrate such a capacity. Thus, the present findings may indicate that Israeli adolescents who are continuously exposed to ongoing terrorism expe- rience an adaptive process. Creative ‘problem solving’ strategies become predominant in dealing effectively with the related stress; for instance, meeting with peers in alternative safe places (e.g. homes, building halls.) rather than shopping malls; organizing parental carpools to joint meeting places rather than using public transportation (Zeidner, 1993, 2005; Pat Horenczyk, Schiff, & Doppelt, 2007). Our second hypothesis was that while ‘non-productive’ coping will be positively linked to the different problems, ‘problem solving’ will be negatively related to these problems. Consistent with this hypothesis our results showed that ‘reference to others’ and ‘non-productive’ (emotion focused) coping are related with more PTS and other emotional and behavioral symptoms. The results are congruent with some research in this field (Zeidner, 2005); however, they conflict with research in the broader context of war which has sometimes indicated strategies such as denial and distancing to be related to fewer symptoms (Muldoon & Cairns, 1999; Weisenberg, Schwarzwald, Waysman, Solomon, & Klingman, 1993). Such differences in results undoubtedly underscore the need to further develop a specific research base as regards dealing with terror- related experiences, given that both appraisal and adaptive coping are likely to differ as a function of the contextual determinants. The present research represents an attempt to examine how adolescents’ mental health outcomes in the context of ongoing terror are affected by situational determinants such as levels of different dimensions of exposure to terror attacks, primary appraisal of perceived danger for self and signif- icant others formulated as subjective exposure, as well as coping strategies. As indicated, given the scant number of published studies on coping strategies in an adolescent population in the context of terror attacks (Cardena et al., 2005; Gil & Caspi, 2006; Wadsworth et al., 2004; Zeidner, 2005), our research ties together important variables which were previously studied in the context of terrorism. These are objective, subjective media exposure and near miss experiences as well as coping strategies to estimate their impact on mental health difficulties. This investigation brings forward the impor- tance of the appraisal of the situation, which in this context is the perceived danger or ‘Subjective Exposure’. Subjective exposure showed the strongest link to ‘non-productive’ and ‘reference to others’ coping strategies in this context. In other words, adolescents who to a larger extent appraised the situation as dangerous, referred more to others, relied on them more for help and worried more about what will happen. Significantly, both ‘negative’ primary appraisal and the above mentioned coping strategies were significantly associated with less well-being, indicating the need to work on these issues when helping adolescents who face long periods of terrorist attacks. On the other hand, problem solving strategies such as being optimistic making time for leisure, keeping fit and healthy etc. were associated with better well being. Such findings in the context of terrorism are in O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 597 line with findings from other domains of psychopathological functioning (Dempsey, 2002; Fryden- berg & Lewis, 1999). Thus, the present findings contain relevant implications for building prevention and/or inter- vention programs for youth dealing with persistent sources of trauma and stress. Both appraisal and coping strategies are subject to guidance; for example through cognitive restructuring. In the long run, working on the reciprocal adjustments of cognitions (how is the situation viewed, what can be done, what can be learned from experiences.) and emotions (feelings of danger, insecu- rity, anger, hope.) with adolescents, individually or in groups, may prove crucial to minimize adverse outcomes and enhance psychological well-being. This has been experienced in other settings of developmental psychopathology through the multi-site FACEÓ program with youth that are dealing with diverse sources of trauma and stress (Celestin & Celestin-Westreich, 2006). Applied to the investigated context, such programs can teach youth which types of appraisal and coping strategies are more likely to minimize psychological distress. Given the present findings, which contribute towards building a growing body of evidence, guidance towards usage of more problem solving strategies could assist adolescents who are facing ongoing risks for terror to be able to manage such sources of stress more effectively. Study limitations and implications for future research When considering interpretation options for our findings, a number of study limitations should be taken into account. Since all data are retrospective self-reports, the extent to which adolescents’ self- reported stress and mental health difficulties correspond to external observations and/or clinical assessment remains to be investigated. Measuring adolescents’ psychological outcomes from multiple perspectives is thus needed to fully assess the implications hereof, especially as regards levels of functional impairment. Furthermore, since we do not have base rate information regarding PTS symptoms and other mental health indicators prior to the study period, we cannot with certainty ascribe the observed outcomes solely to the impact of terrorism. Finally, although the relatively large study sample was chosen to represent different segments of the Israeli adolescent population in terms of location, age range and levels of exposure, generalization of findings is limited. Investigating which elements impact youth’s appraisal (subjective exposure) may provide further insight into associated protective or vulnerability factors. This could be of serve when screening for enrollment in prevention/intervention programs. This seems to be important since subjective exposure such as perceived danger comes forward as one of the important contributors to mental health outcomes in a context of ongoing terror. Finally, ethno-cultural and religious influences may serve as additional sources of vulnerability or resilience, as needs to be assessed in future research. Conclusion With continued universal risks of exposure to terror attacks, Israeli adolescents are obvious candidates for research into the psychological implications of this type of experience. The goal of this investigation has been to explore the use of coping strategies and their relationships to emotional and behavioral problems in adolescents who have been exposed to ongoing terrorism. After three years of ongoing terror attacks Israeli adolescents reported using a wide diversity of 598 O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 coping strategies mainly of problem solving style to deal with the threat of ongoing terror attacks. The different exposure dimensions as well as the different coping strategies, accounted for 26e37% of the variance in exposed adolescents’ post traumatic stress, emotional, behavioral, and related mental health problems. Our findings show that perceived danger (subjective exposure), as well as certain coping strategies, such as, emotional focused coping, bear adverse effects on adolescents’ mental health. In contrast, a more positively oriented appraisal along with problem focused coping may shield protective effects of mental health on adolescents facing a ongoing terrorist attacks. This means that factors referring to the dynamics of the psychological process that is activated when adolescents face ongoing terror- related trauma and stress, (such as subjectively perceived danger and coping) provide important windows of action for prevention and intervention programs to foster adolescents’ well-being. Future research should help sorting out more precisely how primary appraisal, which in its initial stage bears particular relevance for further coping process, maybe articulated against diverse family, community and socio-cultural factors. References Aber, J. L., Gershoff, E. T., Ware, A., & Kotler, J. A. (2004). Estimating the effects of September 11th and other forms of violence on the mental health and social development of New York City’s youth: a matter of context. Applied Developmental Science, 8(3), 111e129. Achenbach, T. M., & Rescola, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families. Aldwin, C. M. (1994). Stress, coping and development: An integrative perspective. New York: Guilford Press. Cardena, E., Dennis, J. M., Winkel, M., & Skitka, L. J. (2005). A snapshot to terror: acute post traumatic response to September 11 attack. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 6(2), 69e84. Celestin, L. P., & Celestin-Westreich, S. (2006). All Equal? Advocating equity in mental health services in France to ethnic-minority disadvantaged youth and their families through the evidence-based FACEÓ protocol. In American Psychiatric Association Annual Meeting 2006, Syllabus & Proceedings Summary (pp. 110e111). Arlington: American Psychiatric Association. Dempsey, M. (2002). Negative coping as mediator in the relation between violence and outcomes: inner-city African American youth. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72(1), 102e109. Derogatis, L. R. (1993). Brief symptom inventory administration, scoring and procedure manual. Minneapolis: NCS Pear- son, Inc.. Frydenberg, E. (1997). Adolescent coping: Theoretical and research prospective. London: Routledge. Frydenberg, E., & Lewis, R. (1993a). Boys play sport and girls turn to others: age gender and ethnicity as determinants of coping. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 252e266. Frydenberg, E., & Lewis, R. (1993b). Adolescent Coping Scale administrator’s manual. Melbourne: The Australian Council for Educational Research Ltd. Frydenberg, E., & Lewis, R. (1999). The Adolescent Coping Scale: construct validity and what the instrument tells us. Australian Journal of Guidance and Counseling, 9, 19e36. Garnefski, N., Legerstee, J., Kraaij, V., Van den Kommer, T., & Teerds, J. (2002). Cognitive coping strategies and symp- toms of depression and anxiety: a comparison between adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 603e611. Gil, S., & Caspi, Y. (2006). Personality traits, coping styles and perceived threat as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder after exposure to a terrorist attack: a perspective study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 904e909. Hoven, C. W., Duarte, C. S., Lucas, C. P., Mandel, D. J., Wu, P., & Rosen, C. (2002). Effects of the world trade center attack on NYC public school students: Initial report of the New York Board of Education. New York: Applied Research and Consulting, LLC & Columbia University Mailman School of Public Health & New York State Psychiatric Institute. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress appraisal and coping. New York: Springer. O. Braun-Lewensohn et al. / Journal of Adolescence 32 (2009) 585e599 599 Lewis, R., & Frydenberg, E. (2002). Concomitants of failure to cope: what we should teach adolescents about coping. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(3), 419e431. Muldoon, O., & Cairns, E. (1999). Children, young people and war: learning to cope. In E. Frydenberg (Ed.), Learning to cope. Developing as a person in complex societies. Oxford University Press. Pat Horenczyk, R. (2003). The impact of ongoing terrorist attacks on the adolescent population in Jerusalem and the Jerusalem area. In M. Caspi (Ed.), Children anxiety as result of the terrorist events. Jerusalem: Israeli Knesset, research and information center. Pat Horenczyk, R., & Doppelt, O. (2005). Screening for post traumatic distress among adolescents who are exposed to on going terror in Israel. In E. Somer, & Bleich. (Eds.), Mental health in terror’s shadow: The Israeli experience. Ramot Press/Tel-Aviv University. Pat Horenczyk, R., Schiff, M., & Doppelt, O. (2007). Maintaining routine despite ongoing exposure to terrorism: a healthy strategy for adolescents. Journal of Adolescence Health, 39(2), 199e205. Pfefferbaum, B., Doughty, D. E., Peddy, C., Patel, N., Gurwithch, R., Nixon, S. J., & Tivis, R. D. (2002). Exposure and peritraumatic response as predictors of postraumatic stress in children following the 1995 Oklahoma city bombing. Journal of Urban Health, 79(3), 354e361. Pfefferbaum, B., Nixon, S. J., Tivis, R. D., Doughty, D. E., Pynoos, R. S., Gurwitch, R. H., et al. (2001). Television exposure in children after a terrorist incident. Psychiatry, 64(3), 202e211. Pfefferbaum, B., Nixon, S., Tucker, P., Tivis, R., Moore, V., Gurwitch, R. H., et al. (1999). Posttraumatic stress responses in bereaved children after the Oklahoma City bombing. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1372e1379. Pfefferbaum, B., North, C. S., Doughty, D. E., Gurwitch, R. H., Fullerton, C. S., & Kyula, J. (2003). Posttraumatic stress and functional impairment in Kenyan children following the 1998 American embassy bombing. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 73(2), 133e140. Pynoss, R. S., & Eth, S. (1985). Children traumatized by witnessing acts of personal violence: Homicide, rape or suicide behavior. In S. Eth, & R. S. Pynoss (Eds.), Post-traumatic stress Disorder in Children. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press. Ronen, T., Rahav, G., & Appel, N. (2003). Adolescent stress responses to a single acute stress and to continuous external stress: terrorist attacks. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 8, 261e282. Solomon, Z., Laufer, A., & Lavi, T. (2005). In the shadow of the Intifada: exposure and post traumatic reactions among adolescents in Israel. In E. Somer, & A. Bleich (Eds.), Mental health in terror’s shadow: The Israeli experience. Ramot Press/Tel-Aviv University. Solomon, Z., & Lavi, T. (2005). Israeli youth in the second Intifada: PTSD and future orientation. Journal of the Amer- ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(11), 1167e1175. Thabet, A. A. M., & Vostanis, P. (2002). Emotional problems in Palestinian children living in a war zone: a cross- sectional study. Lancet, 359, 1801e1804. Wadsworth, M. E., Gudmundsen, R. C., Raviv, T., Ahlkvist, J. A., McIntosh, D. N., Kline, G. H., et al. (2004). Coping with terrorism: age and gender differences in effortful and involuntary responses to September 11th. Applied Devel- opmental Science, 8(3), 143e157. Weisenberg, M., Schwarzwald, J., Waysman, M., Solomon, Z., & Klingman, A. (1993). Coping of school-age children in the sealed room during scud missile bombardment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(3), 462e467. Xianchen, L., Jenn-Yun, T., & Zhongtang, Z. (2004). Coping strategies and behavioral/emotional problems among Chinese adolescents. Psychiatry Research, 126, 275e285. Zeidner, M. (1993). Coping with disaster: the case of Israeli adolescents under threat of missile attack. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22(1), 89e108. Zeidner, M. (2005). Contextual and personal predictors of adaptive outcomes under terror attack: the case of Israeli adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34(5), 459e470.