Research Methods I Week 9 Reliability and Validity PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by Deleted User
Tags
Summary
This document covers reliability and validity in research methods. It explores confounding variables and different types of reliability and validity, such as inter-rater reliability, test-retest reliability, internal consistency, construct validity, content validity, convergent validity, discriminant validity and internal validity.
Full Transcript
**Research Methods I Week 9** **Reliability and Validity** Confounding variables - Uncontrolled 'extraneous' variables that potentially affect the outcome of the study - Major goal of research design is to decrease (or to control) the influence of confounding/ extraneous variables R...
**Research Methods I Week 9** **Reliability and Validity** Confounding variables - Uncontrolled 'extraneous' variables that potentially affect the outcome of the study - Major goal of research design is to decrease (or to control) the influence of confounding/ extraneous variables Reliability - The extent to which your measurement produces consistent and accurate results - An issue for questionnaires or observational designs - Types of reliability: - **Inter-rater reliability**: measures the degree of agreement between different people observing or assessing the same thing - **Test-retest reliability**: measured the consistency of results when you repeat the same test on the same sample at a different point in time - **Internal consistency**: assesses the correlation between multiple items in a test that are intended to measure the same construct Validity - The degree to which a test measures what it intends to measure Construct validity - Is the test measuring what it is supposed to? - 3 different elements: - **Content validity**: does the test measure all aspects of the variable? - **Convergent validity**: does the test give similar results to other measures of the same thing? - **Discriminant validity**: is the test unrelated to measures of other unrelated things? Internal validity - The extent to which the results of an experiment can be attributed to the manipulation of the independent variable (IV) -\> (and not to some uncontrolled variable) - Confounding variables **reduce** internal validity Confounds in between-subjects designs - Different people undergo different conditions of the IV (independent groups) - Problem: non-equivalent groups - E.g. participants choosing particular groups (treatments) based on preference, or other factors - Solution: randomly assign participants to groups - Allows each participant the same chance as all the others of being in a group - Problem with using quasi-independent variables (e.g. age, gender, IQ, ethnicity, handedness, religion) -\> participants may differ in other ways across groups - E.g. compare mood of people who exercise regularly vs people who never exercise -\> groups may also differ in terms of diet, employment, personality traits etc - Solution: **Matching groups** - aim is to match participants between groups on all other relevant variables except the one of interest - often difficult (sometimes impossible): - may not be able to find enough matched participants - may not know what all the relevant variables are Confounds in within-subjects design - each participant takes part in each condition of the IV (repeated measures) - often considered **optimal** because each person is their own control (control for individual differences) - Problem**: order effects** (Carryover effects) - when the order of the tasks affects the DV - practice, fatigue - Solution: **Counterbalancing** - Systematically ordering the conditions for participants - Problem: **maturation effects** - some DVs will change with the passage of time - e.g. study into a new antidepressant: - But mood may have changed overtime anyway, without the antidepressants - Solution: include a **control group** - e.g. give a placebo to some participants Other confounds - **demand characteristics** - when a participant changes their behaviour according to what they think are the expectations of the researchers - e.g. social desirability - solution: deception (ethics?) - unobtrusive manipulation of IV - between-subjects design (so participants are not aware of all levels of IV) Confounds and internal validity - confounding variables can reduce the internal validity of a study - individual differences (between-subjects) - order effects (within-subjects) - demand characteristics (within-subjects) - confounds can be controlled by: - random assignment to groups (between subjects) - matching groups (between subjects, quasi-experimental) - Counterbalancing (within-subjects) Ecological validity - The extent to which the findings of a research study can be generalised to real-life settings External validity - The extent to which the results of a study can be generalised to other situations and to other people - Replication of results by other researchers is a good test of external validity - An important threat to external validity is sampling bias Representativeness - The sample must be representative of the population - Non-representative = biased - Should approximately match the characteristics of the population e.g. gender, age, occupation - Threat to representativeness: - Biased sample = when the characteristics of the sample are different from the population - Response bias = when an unrepresentative section of the population responds to your recruitment advert - Selection bias = when the techniques for selecting the sample lead to over- or under-representation of a proportion of the population Methods of sampling **Probability sampling** - Each member of the population has an equal chance of being selected to participate - Random sampling - Each member of a population is equally likely to be chosen to make up the sample - E.g. if everyone was assigned a number and a random number generator was used to select participants - Systematic sampling - Recruit every Nth person from the population - E.g. recruit every 5^th^ person - Stratified sampling - Ensures that subgroups are represented equally - E.g. 70% female students and 30% male students **Nonprobability sampling** - Each member of the population does not have an equal chance of being selected to take part - Volunteer sampling - The researcher advertises their study and people volunteer to participate - E.g. SONA - Could be biased, as participants are self-selected - Opportunity sampling - Researcher approaches people to participate in their study - E.g. going to a meeting of the Alzheimer's society to recruit people with dementia - Could also be biased - Convenience sampling - People are sampled simply because they are "convenient" sources of data - E.g. collecting data from a research methods class - Could also be biased Sampling in the real world - **Ethical guidelines** state that participants must voluntarily agree to take part - Are volunteers representative of general population? - "volunteerism" may affect external validity - Research has shown that volunteers do not have the same characteristics as the general population (e.g. Rosenthal and Rosnow, 1975) - Very difficult to avoid volunteer bias Sampling and external validity - Sample needs to be representative of the population - A biased sample will reduce the external validity of the study - Random sampling is ideal, but not usually possible - Problems with non-random sampling include volunteer bias