OPT 404 Visual Acuity PDF
Document Details
Uploaded by FeistyOpal8806
University of Plymouth
Tags
Summary
This document provides notes on visual acuity. It discusses concepts such as the visual angle and minimum visible resolution, and explores the influences of factors like the optics of the eye and the visual systems on visual acuity. These notes cover aspects of visual perception and measurement methods.
Full Transcript
OPT 404 notes - Visual Perception Visual Acuity Size and Visual Angle - - The size of the object image on the retina is more important than the physical size of the object - Referencing to the objects size in terms of the visual angle measured in degrees of the visual angle - The image on th...
OPT 404 notes - Visual Perception Visual Acuity Size and Visual Angle - - The size of the object image on the retina is more important than the physical size of the object - Referencing to the objects size in terms of the visual angle measured in degrees of the visual angle - The image on the retina decreases in size with distance from the observer - This can be seen in the diagrams below Different kinds of ACUITY - Minimum visible resolution - DETECTION ACUITY A thin line on background < 1 arc sec (=1/60 arc min); anything less than this results in blurred images on the retina - Minimum angle of resolution (MAR) = RESOLUTION ACUITY Discrimination of a feature; letter optotypes, two point resolution 30-60 arc seconds (6/3 - 6/6) - Minimum discriminable angle = HYPERACUITY Very good - can be 2-10 arc sec If 90%) with a good level of illumination - The smallest spatial detail that can be resolved - Depends on the optics of the eye as well as the visual system - Decreases with increasing eccentricity (in peripheral vision) › - Measure of the spatial resolution ie the smallest letter size that the visual system can identify (5’ letter size for acuity 6/6) TUMBLING E - MAR - Minimal angle of resolution - The smallest spatial detail (gap) that can be resolved (1’ gap size for acuity 6/6) LogMAR and Bailey-Lovie - LogMAR: Logaritm to the base 10 of the minimum angle of resolution (MAR) - The original charts were designed by Bailey and Love in 1976, the alternative being the Thomson Test chart - The 6/60 line gives a score of 10 - Each line below will give a score of 0.1 less than the line above - because of the balanced distribution of the letters which are graded in difficulty as how easy they are to see - For each of the five letters in each line, count for a score of 0.1/5 = 0.02 - As each letter is worth 0.2, if a patient reads on the 0.4 line plus 3 letters of the 0.3 line they will have a score of 0.34 - This is a result of the five letters of line 0.4 minus the score for each letter read from the 0.3 line [0.4 - (0.02 x 3) = 0.34] - NOTE - The 6/6 line is 0.0 logMAR and the next line below where there are smaller letters is -0.1logMAR and the one above where there are larger letters is 0.1logMAR where -Logarithmic progression of size (ie constant factor) is equal perceptual steps - Equal numbers of letters on each line with equal spacing between letters and rows proportional to letter size -Equal average legibility at each acuity line What are other ways of measuring acuity - Patients who are unable to communicate (children etc) - Hundreds and Thousands - hold outside child’s reach, the child grasping at the hundreds and thousands implies an acuity of - 6/12 - 6/24 - Optokinetic Nystagmus - Eyes will naturally follow a moving stimulus and move back - Eg watching road from a car or train - Use rotating drum with different width stripes - OKN only evoked if stripes are more than the minimum angle of resolution - Preferential Looking - 2 AFC (grating either on left or right) - Child will look at interesting/novel stimulus (grating) for longer - Change the width of the stripes (spatial frequency) until they no longer look at the striped region - Average electrical activity in primary visual cortex measured by an EEG or Visual Evoked Potential (VEP) - Present grating to patient, then reduced size until no electrical activity change in response to stimulus What limits Visual Acuity: - OPTICAL LIMITS - Blur (dioptric blur, aberrations, accommodation state) - reduces the contrast of the key features needed to recognise the letter, and the impact of the blur is much greater for small letters than larger ones - Pupil Size (diffraction) - the pupil is adjusted to give the most focussed image, when the light level reduces significantly, there may not be enough light reaching the retina - so the pupil opens wider, sacrificing image quality for increased light hitting the retina - small pupils increase VA, which improved discrimination of fine stimuli - and large pupils increase light influx which improved detection of faint stimuli - NEURAL LIMITS - Sampling density (cone mosaic) - Luminance level - Eccentricity - Adaptation state - EXTERNAL LIMITS (stimulus characteristics or other extrinsic factors) - Luminance level - Constrast - Orientation - Age - Amblyopia (lazy eye) - Binocularity - focusing on one object with both eyes Chromatic Aberration - Different wavelengths are refracted differently - Longitudinal chromatic aberration means that if the eye is focused on one wavelength, it is blurred for others - Chromatic Aberration is significant in human optics but we do not notice it - Longitudinal Chromatic Aberration is used in the DUOCHROME TEST - used to finesse optical correction - Green and red lights should be equally blurred or clear so that prescription is best for light at the peak of wavelength (YELLOW) The circle of least confusion - the point at which the image is in the most focus as the rays do not fall perfectly to a point, rather than a small area Bichromatic tests (duochrome) assume that the visual system focuses yellow light (570nm) This is for an emmetropic eye - perfect 20/20 vision Hyperopia - LONG SIGHTEDNESS Myopia - SHORT SIGHTEDNESS Stiles-Crawford Effect - The phenomenon in which light entering the eye near the centre of the pupil appears brighter than light entering near the edge Optical Limits to Acuity - Diffraction and Aberration - Both cause blur - The extent of this is primarily dependant on pupil size - The relationship between blur and pupil size is opposite for these two caused of optical blur - Diffraction is a result of the wave nature of light - it is unavoidable in any optical system that has an aperture - When light passes through a small aperture (eg pupil) it spreads out behind it - The amount of spread depends on the size of the aperture - Using the pinhole aperture to cut out refractive error is sometimes clinically useful - When this is used, aberrations and refractive errors will be minimised allowing the clinician to tell if a reduction in visual acuity is due to neural factors rather than optical factors Neural Limits to acuity: Resolution of 2 points - To distinguish or resolve the two points, the retina must be able to measure the light level at 3 points: both peaks and the trough - There has to be one cone in between, which is not stimulated to resolve 2 spots MAR: cone spacing versus optical quality - Sampling of the image by the photoreceptors, and by subsequent retinal structures (bipolar cells and retinal ganglion cells) must be fine enough to capture the information provided by the image - Three receptors are in priniciple required to distinguish gap in eg Snellen Letter Eccentricity - Acuity will increase as eccentricity gets closer to 0 degrees (minus figures in the nasal field and positive figures in the temporal field) Differences in foveal and peripheral vision - MAR increases with retinal eccentricity - In the periphery: - Cone spacing increases - Spatial pooling of cone responses increases - Ganglion cell receptive field size increases - Foveal and peripheral vision are similar, the main difference is a change in the scale at which the visual system operates - At the fovea, the receptors sampling density is just fine enough to capture the fine detail that the eye’s optics can deliver - Any finer would be wasteful; any coarser would be ‘undersample’ the image and lead to loss of image information Luminance - Scotopic and photopic parts of the curve - Convergence onto retinal ganglion cells decreases with increasing luminance - Above -85cm/m2 OK Orientation - Acuity is better for vertical and horizontal than for oblique stimuli - More V1 neurones tuned to vertical and horizontal than to oblique angles - The so-called oblique effect Age - Very young and old have poorer acuity - 6/120 (MAR = 20’) at birth to adult levels at age 2 - Due to increased absorption of light by ocular media and decrease in photoreceptor function Amblyopia (lazy eye) - A reduced VA in one eye not correctable with prescription and no disease - Neural problem: disruption of visual information transfer to one eye (the amblyopic eye) to the brain - Usually caused by lack of appropriate visual input during critical period - Crowding effect is worse in amblyopia - Do not use single letters in children as you might miss an amblyopic eye - Preferably use logMAR chart or the crowding bars or box around the letters when using single letters Visual Deprivation - Abnormal early visual experience resulting in possible permanent consequences - Monocular vision from deprivation can cause massive changes in cortical physiology, resulting in devastating and permanent loss of spatial vision - Cataracts and strabismus (crossed eyes) can lead to serious problems but early detection and care can prevent such problems Binocularity - Binocular acuity usually better than monocular acuity - Probability Summation: Two eyes better than one, but NOT twice as good - Better by a factor of 1.4 (Square root of 2 - binocular summation) - If binocular VA is worse than monocular VA, suspect an uncompensated heterophoria or an unbalanced refractive error Minimum discriminable angle - Hyperacuity - localisation acuity - Easy to support we can’t distinguish 2 points because we can’t tell their locations apart - This is not true - we are much better at locating a spot than resolving 2 spots - 2 point resoltion is about 30” of arc - Positional accuracy is about 2” of arc, under ideal conditions - Positional accuracy can be measured using Verniers acuity experiments - Robust against optical degradation (blur from defocus or cataract) so good to assess the neural function - Below see Verniers acuity stimulus examples Is visual Acuity the only important measure of ability to see things? - NO - Acuity measures the ability to see small, high contrast features - It is measured because it is the main thing you can help with, but loss of acuity is usually caused by the optics of the eye - But vision rarely operates at high contrast levels - For instance, fog, rain, light levels affect contrast UNKNOWN WORDS - see underline ARC SEC - A circle is 360 degrees, each degree is split into 60 minutes and each minute is split into 60 seconds - an arc is a portion of the circumference of a circle So an arc sec is 1/3600 of a degree, in reference to ’20/20’ vision or 6/6 vision - means that visual acuity can resolve down to one arc minute - 1/60 of a degree Used to measure very small angles LETTER OPTOTYPES - Think Snellen chart etc, letters or symbols at a controlled distance and size to measure visual acuity CONE SPACING - The distance between cones in the retina